Sports and Exercise Medicine

Open journal

ISSN 2379-6391

Prevalence of Eating Disorders among College Female Cheerleaders and Non-Athletes

Judy R. Wilson* and Angela Liegey-Dougall

Judy R. Wilson, PhD

Associate Professor, Department of Kinesiology, University of Texas at Arlington, Arlington, TX 76019, USA; E-mail: jrwilson@uta.edu

INTRODUCTION

The sporting world has been aware of disordered eating and eating disorders for some time.1,2 It has also been determined that sports, where leanness or a specific weight is needed for better sports performance, have a higher risk of developing eating disorders than athletes in team sports and the general population.3 This appears to be true for both male and female athletes in sports that rely on a lean body shape or low weight.4 It has also been determined that disordered eating is more prevalent among athletes than non-athletes.5 The concern for those with eating disorders is that they are “complex illnesses with profound psychosocial and physical consequences, including high rates or mortality. Despite growing recognition of their prevalence and severity, eating disorders remain underdiagnosed and undertreated”.6 Furthermore, eating disorders, like all mental health conditions, exist on a continuum of symptom severity from clinically severe to subthreshold.7 Subthreshold eating disorders are defined by clinically significant eating disordered behaviors that do not meet diagnostic criteria for clinical eating disorders, however, they often progress and reach the threshold for eating disorders.8

Competitive cheerleading is considered comparable to other lean sports such as gymnastics, ballet, swimming, and diving all of which have a long history of increased risk of eating disorders.9 In the past, the primary responsibility of cheerleaders was to assist in crowd enthusiasm and promote school events during the academic year. Today, cheerleading teams are made up of members from a gym or clubs who compete throughout the year. College cheerleaders generally appear at many events but only have one annual competition judged by a panel of judges.10 Squads consist of base and back spots who toss, catch, and hold the “flyers”. Of necessity, they are larger and stronger while the flyers are generally shorter, lighter, and have lower body mass indexes (BMI) compared to other positions. In a study using the eating attitudes test (EAT-26), the prevalence of those meeting the diagnostic criteria of disordered eating risk was 33%.11 At our university, the Cheer Team competes at the National Cheerleaders Association (NCA) collegiate National Championships (Small Co-ed Division 1) every year and have won six national championships since 2010. We hypothesized that members of the Cheer Team might exhibit symptoms of eating disorders and were recruited for this study. Unfortunately, we were unable to separate them by position.

College students, in general, are an important population to consider for prevention and early intervention for eating disorders. The traditional college years (age 18 to 25-years) directly coincide with the median age of onset for eating disorders.12 It has been estimated that 11% to 17% of females and approximately 4% of males on college campuses in the United States of America (U.S.A.) screen positive for clinical eating disorder symptoms.13 However, participants for control groups were recruited from undergraduate majors who, it was hypothesized, would have lower instances of eating disorders than the estimates.

The American Kinesiology Association (AKA) defines kinesiology as an academic discipline that involves the study of physical activity and its impact on health, society, and quality of life.14 The emphasis on health and physical activity suggests that few kinesiology students would experience problems with eating disorders. Participants were also recruited from undergraduate psychology majors. It was hypothesized that the education and training received would be reflected in positive mental health.15

The purpose of this study was to measure symptoms of eating disorders that might be present in female undergraduate students from specific disciplines.

METHODS

Female participants from the University’s Cheerleading squad, an introductory Psychology class, and an introductory Kinesiology course were recruited to participate in this study that was approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) for the Protection of Human Subjects. They reported to a classroom in groups of 20 and were given as much time as they needed to complete the eating disorder inventory-2 (EDI-2).16

Measures

Age, height, and weight were recorded on the questionnaires and BMI was calculated by dividing self-reported weight in pounds by self-reported height in inches squared and then multiplying by 703.

Eating disorder inventory-2 scales: To assess eating disorder symptoms, the following scales were created by summing their respective items: drive for thinness (Cronbach’s α=0.91), bulimia (Cronbach’s α=0.42), body dissatisfaction (Cronbach’s α=0.87), ineffectiveness (Cronbach’s α=0.84), perfectionism (Cronbach’s α=0.72), interpersonal distrust (Cronbach’s α=0.70), introceptive awareness (Cronbach’s α=0.77), maturity fears (Cronbach’s α=0.75), asceticism (Cronbach’s α=0.47), impulse regulation (Cronbach’s α=0.85), and social insecurity (Cronbach’s α=0.79).

RESULTS

Data Analyses

The frequency distributions and descriptive statistics for the variables were examined first. Square root transformations were conducted on seven of the 11 EDI-2 scales to correct for positively skewed distributions: drive for thinness, bulimia, ineffectiveness, interpersonal distrust, interoceptive awareness, impulse regulation, and social insecurity. Group differences in age and BMI were examined using analyses of variance (ANOVAs). Mean differences among the groups in the EDI-2 scales were examined using multivariate analyses of covariance (MANCOVAs). Bonferroni adjustments were made to all post-hoc tests. Relationships between group membership and other categorical variables of interest were examined with chi-square tests of independence. A comparison of prevalence rates of high-risk status for eating disorders to college norms was done using chi-square (χ2) goodness of fit statistics.

Participants

A total of 86 female participants completed the survey. There were 27 university Cheerleaders who were compared to 30 students enrolled in introductory Psychology courses, and 29 students enrolled in introductory Kinesiology courses.

The mean age of the sample was 20.58 years (SD=0.21; range=18-30). The Cheerleader group (M=19.33, SD=0.44) was younger than the Kinesiology control group (M=21.66, SD=0.42), but neither group differed from the Psychology control group (M=20.67, SD=0.42), F (2, 83) =75.71, p=0.001, ηp 2 =0.15. The Cheerleader (M=21.30, SD=0.51), Psychology (M=21.64, SD=0.49), and Kinesiology groups all had comparable BMI means (M=22.73, SD=0.49), F(2, 83)=2.23, p=0.11, ηp 2 =0.05. Age and BMI were evaluated as covariates in all of the models presented below.

EDI-2 Scales

It was expected that Cheerleaders would report higher EDI-2 scale scores than either the Psychology or Kinesiology control groups. To test this, participants’ scores on each of the 11 subscales of the EDI-2 were compared across the three groups covarying for age and BMI using a MANCOVA. There was a significant multivariate main effect for group, multi. F (22, 138)=1.83, p=0.02, ηp 2 =0.23. Raw score means and standard errors were presented in Table 1.

Table 1. Means, standard errors, and univariate tests for each of the EDI-2 Subscales
EDI-2 Scale Cheerleader M (SE) Psychology M (SE) Kinesiology M (SE) F(2,79) p Partial η2
Drive for Thinness† 6.17 (1.18)a 6.25 (1.10) 3.35 (1.13)a 3.93 0.02 0.09
Bulimia† 2.11 (0.35)a 1.04 (0.33) 0.69 (0.34)a 4.03 0.02 0.09
Body Dissatisfaction 10.47 (1.25)a 8.30 (1.16) 5.08 (1.20)a 4.55 0.01 0.10
Ineffectiveness† 3.06 (0.72) 2.87 (0.67) 0.70 (0.69) 3.62 0.03 0.08
Perfectionism 7.14 (0.89) 6.36 (0.83) 7.95 (0.86) 0.88 0.42 0.02
Interpersonal Distrust† 2.20 (0.54) 3.18 (0.50)a 1.27 (0.51)a 3.09 0.05 0.07
Interoceptive Awareness† 3.25 (0.73) 2.83 (0.68) 1.62 (0.70) 2.00 0.14 0.05
Maturity Fears 5.27 (0.73)a 4.68 (0.68)b 2.02 (0.70)a,b 5.54 0.01 0.12
Asceticism 4.28 (0.51) 3.06 (0.48) 3.40 (0.49) 1.55 0.22 0.04
Impulse Regulation† 3.98 (0.99) 3.32 (0.92) 1.09 (0.95) 2.91 0.06 0.07
Social Insecurity† 3.01 (0.66) 3.52 (0.62) 1.94 (0.64) 1.04 0.36 0.03
Energy intake from supplement (kcal/mice/day) 0.00±0.00 0.08±0.00 0.15±0.01 0.39±0.02 0.39±0.02 0.39±0.02
Water intake (mL/mice/day) 7.0±0.2 7.0±0.2 7.0±0.1 7.0±0.3 7.0±0.3 7.0±0.3
† Means and standard errors were for raw data and the F tests were for the square root transformed data.
Note: Means with the same superscript were significantly different using Bonferroni adjustment and controlling for age and body mass index.

As expected, the Cheerleader group scored higher than the Kinesiology control group on the drive for thinness, bulimia, body dissatisfaction, and maturity fears. The psychology control group also scored higher than the Kinesiology control group on the interpersonal distrust and maturity fears scales. Contrary to expectations there were no group differences for ineffectiveness, perfectionism, interoceptive awareness, asceticism, impulse regulation, and social insecurity.

Additionally, the EDI-2 cut-off score for high-risk was created using a score greater than or equal to 14 on the drive for thinness scale.16 Contrary to expectations, prevalence rates for students who were at high-risk were not associated with group membership, χ2 (2, N=85)=0.58, p=0.75. Approximately, 15% (n=4) of the Cheerleader sample, 17% (n=5) of the psychology control group, and 10% (n=3) of the Kinesiology control group were classified as at high-risk for an eating disorder. χ2 (1, N=27)=0.69, p=0.40; χ2 (1, N=29)=1.69, p=0.19; χ2 (1, N=29)=0.004, p=0.95; respectively. 

Eating Disorder Behaviors

Participants reported on whether they engaged in a number of behaviors associated with disordered eating during the previous three months (Table 2).

Table 2. Prevalence of Eating Disorder Behaviors during the Previous Three Months
Eating Behavior Cheerleader n (%) Psychology n (%) Kinesiology n (%) N χ2 p
Binge No 23 (85%) 23 (82%) 24 (83%) 84 0.10 0.95
Yes 4 (15%) 5 (18%) 5 (17%)
Purge No 18 (69%) 24 (89%) 24 (83%) 82 3.14 0.18
Yes 8 (31%) 3 (11%) 5 (17%)
Laxatives No 24 (89%) 28 (97%) 27 (93%) 85 1.25 0.53
Yes 3 (11%) 1 (3%) 2 (7%)
Diet Pills No 18 (69%) 21 (72%) 21 (72%) 84 0.09 0.96
Yes 8 (31%) 8 (28%) 8 (28%)
Diuretics No 26 (96%) 28 (97%) 28 (97%) 85 0.004 1.00
Yes 1 (4%) 1 (3%) 1 (3%)

 

Contrary to expectations, group was not associated with any of these behaviors. The eating disorder behaviors that were the most prevalent were use of diet pills, purging, and binging. To test the hypothesis that Cheerleaders who engaged in these behaviors would report more symptoms of disordered eating than control participants who engaged in the same behaviors, MANCOVAs were conducted using each of these three behaviors as a moderating factor of group membership. Use of diet pills did not significantly moderate the effects of group on the EDI-2 scales, multi. F(22, 128)=1.32, p=0.17, ηp 2 =0.18. However, purging and binging significantly interacted with group, multi. F(22, 124)=1.74, p=0.03, ηp 2 =0.24 and multi. F(22, 128)=2.11, p=0.005, ηp 2 =0.27, respectively (Tables 3 and 4).a

Table 3. Means, Standard Errors, and Univariate Tests for the Interaction between Group and Purging for each of the EDI-2 Scales
EDI-2 Scale Purge Cheerleader M (SE) Psychology M (SE) Kinesiology M (SE) F(2,72) p Partial η2
Drive for Thinness† No 3.87 (1.30) 5.60 (1.14) 2.99 (1.12) 1.18 0.31 0.03
Yes 12.53 (1.96) 12.06 (3.09) 4.68 (2.45)
Bulimia† No 1.68 (0.41) 0.80 (0.36) 0.61 (0.36) 0.58 0.56 0.02
Yes 3.44 (0.62) 2.45 (0.98) 0.92 (0.78)
Body Dissatisfaction No 7.81 (1.35) 7.68 (1.19) 4.78 (1.16) 2.86 0.06 0.07
Yes 17.69 (2.04)a 14.87 (3.21) 5.98 (2.55)a
Ineffectiveness† No 2.19 (0.82) 2.32 (0.72) 0.65 (0.71) 1.33 0.27 0.04
Yes 5.75 (1.24) 3.54 (1.96) 0.48 (1.55)
Perfectionism No 7.12 (1.08) 5.67 (0.96) 7.92 (0.94) 1.23 0.30 0.03
Yes 7.38 (1.64) 10.68 (2.58) 7.90 (2.05)
Interpersonal Distrust† No 1.57 (0.64) 2.97 (0.56) 1.32 (0.55) 0.89 0.42 0.02
Yes 3.79 (0.97) 4.10 (1.52) 0.85 (1.21)
Interoceptive Awareness† No 2.09 (0.82) 2.28 (0.72) 1.64 (0.71) 2.13 0.13 0.06
Yes 6.59 (1.24) 4.79 (1.95) 0.92 (1.55)
Maturity Fears No 4.67 (0.85) 4.50 (0.75) 2.00 (0.74) 1.13 0.33 0.03
Yes 7.51 (1.29) 4.20 (2.03) 1.88 (1.61)
Asceticism No 4.39 (0.58) 2.72 (0.52) 3.11 (0.51) 0.34 0.71 0.01
Yes 4.64 (0.89) 3.10 (1.39) 4.63 (1.11)
Impulse Regulation† No 2.57 (0.98) 2.79 (0.86) 0.53 (0.84) 3.42 0.04 0.09
Yes 7.94 (1.48)a 0.08 (2.32)a 3.16 (1.84)
Social Insecurity† No 2.16 (0.72) 3.01 (0.63) 1.54 (0.62) 0.33 0.72 0.01
Yes 5.12 (1.09) 3.40 (1.71) 3.56 (1.36)
† Means and standard errors were for raw data and the F tests were for the square root transformed data.
Note: Means with the same superscript were significantly different using Bonferroni adjustment and controlling for age and body mass index.

 

a. The interaction terms were the main analyses of interest and qualified any of the significant main effects that were observed in these models. However, there was a significant main effect for group, multi. F(22, 124)=2.06, p=0.01, ηp 2 =0.27, but not a main effect for purging, multi. F(11, 62)=1.66, p=0.10, ηp 2 =0.23, in the model containing the interaction between group and purging. The pattern of findings for the group main effect matched those presented in Table 1 with the following exceptions: there was an additional univariate effect for Interoceptive Awareness in which Psychology students reported greater awareness than Kinesiology students; Cheerleaders reported more Ineffectiveness than did Kinesiology students; and Psychology students no longer differed from Kinesiology students on Maturity Fears. In the model containing the interaction between group and binging, there were significant main effects for binging, multi. F(11, 64)=3.22, p=0.002, ηp 2 =0.36, and group, multi. F(22, 128)=2.97, p<0.001, ηp 2 =0.34. For the main effect of binging, students who binged reported more symptoms associated with Drive for Thinness, Bulimia, Body Dissatisfaction, Ineffectiveness, Interpersonal Distrust, Interoceptive Awareness, Impulse Regulation, and Social Insecurity than did students who did not binge. The pattern of findings for the group main effect was similar to those presented in Table 1. There was an additional univariate effect for Interoceptive Awareness in which Cheerleaders reported more symptoms than did the Kinesiology students. There were also several post-hoc effects that emerged, such as Cheerleaders reported more Body Dissatisfaction, Ineffectiveness, and Impulse Regulation than did the Kinesiology group, Cheerleaders no longer differed from Kinesiology students on Maturity Fears, and Psychology students no longer differed from Kinesiology students on Interpersonal Distrust.><0.001, ηp 2 =0.34. For the main effect of binging, students who binged reported more symptoms associated with Drive for Thinness, Bulimia, Body Dissatisfaction, Ineffectiveness, Interpersonal Distrust, Interoceptive Awareness, Impulse Regulation, and Social Insecurity than did students who did not binge. The pattern of findings for the group main effect was similar to those presented in Table 1. There was an additional univariate effect for Interoceptive Awareness in which Cheerleaders reported more symptoms than did the Kinesiology students. There were also several post-hoc effects that emerged, such as Cheerleaders reported more Body Dissatisfaction, Ineffectiveness, and Impulse Regulation than did the Kinesiology group, Cheerleaders no longer differed from Kinesiology students on Maturity Fears, and Psychology students no longer differed from Kinesiology students on Interpersonal Distrust.

Table 4. Means, Standard Errors, and Univariate Tests for the Interaction between Group and Binging for each of the EDI-2 Scales
EDI-2 Scale Binge Cheerleader M (SE) Psychology M (SE) Kinesiology M (SE) F(2,72) p Partial η2
Drive for Thinness† No 4.51 (1.09) 5.63 (1.11) 3.05 (1.06) 4.17 0.02 0.10
Yes 17.20 (2.59)a,b 6.69 (2.26)a 4.02 (2.38)b
Bulimia† No 1.45 (0.31) 0.97 (0.32) 0.42 (0.30) 5.20 0.01 0.12
Yes 6.23 (0.74)a,b 1.26 (0.64)a 1.80 (0.68)b
Body Dissatisfaction No 9.11 (1.20)a 7.15 (1.22) 4.64 (1.16)a 2.68 0.7 0.07
Yes 19.84 (2.84)a,b 10.23 (2.48)a 6.39 (2.62)b
Ineffectiveness† No 2.62 (0.75) 2.19 (0.76) 0.65 (0.71) 0.47 0.63 0.01
Yes 6.10 (1.76) 6.28 (1.54) 1.51 (1.62)
Perfectionism No 6.46 (0.91) 5.74 (0.92) 7.92 (0.88) 1.65 0.20 0.04
Yes 11.75 (2.16) 7.31 (1.88) 7.55 (1.98)
Interpersonal Distrust† No 1.91 (0.56) 2.70 (0.56) 1.03 (0.54) 0.16 0.85 0.004
Yes 4.36 (1.31) 5.10 (1.15) 2.15 (1.21)
Interoceptive Awareness† No 1.92 (0.66) 2.74 (0.67) 1.58 (0.64) 6.21 0.003 0.14
Yes 11.74 (1.56)a 3.38 (1.36) 1.11 (1.44)a
Maturity Fears No 5.41 (0.77) 4.20 (0.78) 2.03 (0.74) 2.14 0.12 0.06
Yes 4.57 (1.81) 8.27 (1.58) 2.04 (1.67)
Asceticism No 3.98 (0.55) 3.07 (0.56) 3.20 (0.53) 0.92 0.40 0.02
Yes 6.04 (1.30) 2.66 (1.13) 4.32 (1.20)
Impulse Regulation† No 2.20 (0.86) 2.64 (0.87) 0.45 (0.84) 4.31 0.02 0.10
Yes 15.58 (2.04)a,b 6.09 (1.78)a 3.24 (1.88)b
Social Insecurity† No 2.28 (0.67) 3.22 (0.68) 1.55 (0.65) 0.70 0.50 0.02
Yes 7.71 (1.58) 5.11 (1.38) 3.51 (1.46)
† Means and standard errors were for raw data and the F tests were for the square root transformed data.
Note: Means with the same superscript were significantly different using Bonferroni adjustment and controlling for age and body mass index.

 

As expected, among participants who purged in the past three months, Cheerleaders reported more Body Dissatisfaction than the Kinesiology control group and more Impulse Regulation than the Psychology control group. Additionally, among participants who binged in the past three months, Cheerleaders reported greater drive for thinness, bulimia, body dissatisfaction, and impulse regulation than both the Psychology and Kinesiology control groups and greater Interoceptive Awareness than the Kinesiology control group. Among participants who did not binge, Cheerleaders also reported greater body dissatisfaction than the Kinesiology control group.

DISCUSSION

The purpose of this study was to examine symptoms of disordered eating among female college Cheerleaders in comparison to two different college samples (Kinesiology and Psychology) considered controls. These two groups were chosen as it was hypothesized that the curriculum would teach healthy behaviors, both physical and mental, respectively. While the size of the samples was small (n≤30), the results revealed that the current samples were comparable to expected college norms regarding risk status for eating disorders. Partial support for the hypothesis that the Cheerleader group would report more symptoms of disordered eating was found. The Cheerleader group reported greater drive for thinness, bulimia, body dissatisfaction, and maturity fears than the Kinesiology control group. Contrary to expectations, the Psychology control group also reported greater interpersonal distrust and maturity fears than the Kinesiology group. Although group membership was not associated with any of the eating disorder behaviors that were measured, the hypothesis that Cheerleaders who engaged in disordered eating behavior would report greater symptoms of eating disorders than the control students who engaged in the same behavior was partially supported for purging and binging. Cheerleaders who purged reported greater body dissatisfaction than the Kinesiology students who purged and greater impulse regulation than psychology students who purged. Cheerleaders who binged in the past three months reported greater drive for thinness, bulimia, body dissatisfaction, and impulse regulation than both the Psychology and Kinesiology students who binged. They also reported greater interoceptive awareness than the Kinesiology control group, and Cheerleaders who did not binge reported greater body dissatisfaction than the Kinesiology students who did not binge. However, members of the Cheer Team may perceive their disordered eating behaviors as part of their sports participation rather than a problem with eating behaviors.

The findings that 17% of the Psychology control group and 10% of the Kinesiology control group had symptoms that classified them as at high-risk for an eating disorder are in line with those of the females on college campuses (11-17%) who screen positive for clinical eating disorder symptoms.13 However, the Cheer Team sample in this study indicated that 15% had symptoms that put them at risk for an eating disorder which was well-below that found in an earlier study (33%).11 A limitation in this study was the lack of differentiation as to Cheer Team position (i.e. base, flyer, etc.) that should be investigated in future studies.

CONCLUSION

Eating disorders continue to be of great concern, for children and adolescents through adulthood.17 Colleges are the last educational institution that most young adults attend before entering college and may represent a place for the recognition of these symptoms and provide appropriate interventions to reduce the symptoms of eating disorders.18 Unfortunately, individuals with disordered eating habits rarely seek help and those few who do so often do not reveal much information that would be useful. Since most methods for researching eating disorders rely on self-reports and interviews, it is easy for these individuals to hide.19 The findings of such a low response for symptoms of eating disorders in the Cheer Team may have been due to those who chose not to volunteer. Continued study of subthreshold symptoms that do not meet the diagnostic criteria8 and identifying non-adaptive stress coping methods may lead to early intervention for both subthreshold and clinical groups.18 Adding participants from other areas of study in undergraduate as well as graduate curricula will help expand the understanding of stresses that may contribute to disordered eating behaviour’s.

PARTICIPANTS’ CONSENT

The informed consent document was approved by the Institutional Review Board for the Protection of Human Subjects at our university.

CONFLICTS OF INTEREST

The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

1. Bratland-Sanda S, Sundgot-Borgen J. Eating disorders in athletes: Overview of prevalence, risk factors and recommendations for prevention and treatment. Eur J Sport Sci. 2013; 13(5): 499-508. doi: 10.1080/17461391.2012.740504

2. ACSM. Position stand the female athlete triad. Journal of Dance Medicine & Science. 1998; 2(1): 40-41. doi: 10.1177/1089313X9800200107

3. Cook BJ, Hausenblas HA. Eating disorder-specific health-related quality of life and exercise in college females. Qual Life Res. 2011; 20(9): 1385-1390. doi: 10.1007/s11136-011-9879-6

4. Byrne S, McLean N. Elite athletes: Effects of the pressure to be thin. J Sci Med Sport. 2002; 5(2): 80-94. doi: 10.1016/s1440-2440(02)80029-9

5. Sundgot-Borgen J, Torstveit MK. Prevalence of eating disorders in elite athletes is higher than in the general population. Clin J Sport Med. 2004; 14(1): 25-32. doi: 10.1097/00042752-200401000-00005

6. Rome ES, Strandjord SE. Eating disorders. Pediatr Rev. Aug. 2016; 37(8): 323-36. doi: 10.1542/pir.2015-0180

7. Lipson SK, Sonneville KR. Eating disorder symptoms among undergraduate and graduate students at 12 U.S. colleges and universities. Eat Behav. 2017; 24: 81-88. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2016.12.003

8. Brand-Gothelf A, Leor S, Apter A, Fennig S. The impact of comorbid depressive and anxiety disorders on severity of anorexia nervosa in adolescent girls. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2014; 202(10): 759-762. doi: 10.1097/nmd.0000000000000194

9. Smith AB, Gay JL, Arent SM, Sarzynski MA, Emerson DM, Torres-cGehee TM. Examination of the prevalence of female athlete triad components among competitive cheerleaders. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2022; 19(3): 1375. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19031375

10. Smith AB, Gay JL, Monsma EV, et al. Investigation of eating disorder risk and body image dissatisfaction among female competitive cheerleaders. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2022; 19(4): 2196. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19042196

11. Torres-McGehee TM, Monsma EV, Dompier TP, Washburn SA. Eating disorder risk and the role of clothing in collegiate cheerleaders’ body images. J Athl Train. 2012; 47(5): 541-548. doi: 10.4085/1062-6050-47.5.03

12. Franko DL, Keel PK. Suicidality in eating disorders: Occurrence, correlates, and clinical implications. Clin Psychol Rev. 2006; 26(6): 769-782. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.04.001

13. Eisenberg D, Nicklett EJ, Roeder K, Kirz NE. Eating disorder symptoms among college students: Prevalence, persistence, correlates, and treatment-seeking. J Am Coll Health. 2011; 59(8): 700-707. doi: 10.1080/07448481.2010.546461

14. Association AK. Website. www.americankinesiology.org/. Accessed December 11, 2023.

15. American Psychological Association (APA). Guidelines for the undergraduate psychology major: Version 2.0. Am Psychol. 2016; 71(2): 102-111. doi: 10.1037/a0037562

16. Garner D. Eating Disorder Inventory2: Professional Manual. Odessa, India: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1991.

17. Hornberger LL, Lane MA. Identification and management of eating disorders in children and adolescents. Pediatrics. 2021; 147(1): e2020040279. doi: 10.1542/peds.2020-040279

18. Miyake Y, Okamoto Y, Takagaki K, Yoshihara M. Changes in eating attitudes and risk for developing disordered eating behaviors in college students with subthreshold eating disorders: A cohort study. Psychopathology. 2023; 56(4): 276-284. doi: 10.1159/000527604

19. McLester C, Hardin R, Hoppe S. Susceptibility to eating disorders among collegiate female student–athletes. J Athl Train. 2014; 49(3): 406-410. doi: 10.4085/1062-6050-49.2.16

LATEST ARTICLES

Chest X-ray Showed a Hazy Left Upper Lung Infiltrate

A Noteworthy Case of Myasthenic Crisis Induced by Levofloxacin

Ada Young*, Ramya Ramesh and Milind Awale

doi.

The Right Thigh Anterior Compartment was Swollen, and the Skin was Ulcerated due to the Traditional Cautery

Primary Skeletal Muscle Lymphoma: A Case Report and Literature Review

Solomon Bishaw*, Addisu Alemu and Abel Tefera

doi.

An Unusual Presentation of Encephalitis in a Patient with Lyme Neuroborreliosis

Maithily Patel*, Jazmin Jatana, Ramya Ramesh and Milind Awale

doi.

Practical Pointers for Drug Development and Medical Affairs

Gerald L. Klein*, Roger E. Morgan, Shabnam Vaezzadeh, Burak Pakkal and Pavle Vukojevic

doi.

10.17140/CTPOJ-7-125

Prevalence and Risk Factors of Subclinical Mastitis of Goats in Banadir Region, Somalia

Omar M. Salah*, Yasin H. Sh-Hassan, Moktar O. S. Mohamed, Mohamed A. Yusuf and Abas S. A. Jimale

doi.10.17140/VMOJ-9-184

LATEST ARTICLES