Psychology and Cognitive Sciences

Open journal

ISSN 2380-727X

Moral Reasoning in Driving Behavior

Leon J. James*

Leon J. James, PhD

Professor, Department of Psychology, University of Hawaii at Manoa, 2500 Campus Rd, Honolulu, HI 96822, USA; E-mail: leon@hawaii.edu

Driving has become stressful, dangerous, and demeaning. Drivers report being stressed out, threatened by each other, exhibit bad moods, terrorize their passengers, and often fantasize violent acts against other motorists or bicyclists.13 These serious issues indicate that there is a strong need for driving psychology and education that can help reverse this trend and change people’s driving habits. Drivers need to be taught the moral dimension of driving, which indicates the character we have as a driver.4,5 Research is needed to assess how drivers can be taught to assess their own driving personality as supportive or hostile, rational or impatient, calm or frustrated, error-free or making mistakes, and cooperative or opportunistic. There is a need for motorists to acknowledge that driving is a social activity that requires coordinated interactions.6

The recent dramatic spread of violence expressed with vehicles indicates the reluctance of drivers to scrutinize their own conduct, preferring to blame other drivers.7,8 Drivers tend to have an inflated self-image of their motoring ability, rating the safety of their own driving as much better than the average motorist’s. For instance, 2 out of 3 drivers rate themselves as almost perfect in how excellent they are as a driver (9 or 10 on a 10-point scale), while the rest consider themselves above average (6 to 8). Survey polls show that 70% of the drivers report about being a victim of an aggressive driver, while only 30% admit being aggressive drivers.9 This suggests that most drivers overlook their own faults, and overestimate their own competence.

Interestingly, drivers who consider their own driving to be near perfect, also confess to significantly more aggressiveness than drivers who see themselves as still improving.9 This reveals the lack of objectivity in self-assessment shown by every 2 out of the 3 drivers. Despite their self-confessed aggressiveness, they still insist on thinking of themselves as near perfect drivers with almost no room to improve. This egocentric phenomenon can be seen in specific forms of aggressive behavior. For example, those who see themselves as near perfect drivers, admit to twice as much of chasing of other cars compared to those who see themselves as less perfect. The difference: 15% vs. 8% is statistically significant.9 The fact is clear: a part of being an aggressive driver is to deny that you need to improve. This indicates a resistance to change.

Wickens et al10 found a moderate relationship between the scores on the “Driver Vengeance Questionnaire” and personality factors such as “narcissism” and “trait driver stress”. Drivers who are vengeful feel that it is warranted for them to punish another driver who deserves it. James and Nahl11 describe this attitude as the “vigilante” role that some drivers play out when driving. Bumgarner et al12 report that drivers who score higher on a “forgiveness” measure and are less likely to express anger against other drivers, are less likely to behave aggressively.

Bianchi & Summala13 report significant correlations between driving style of parents and their adolescent children. Part of this co-variance may be due to shared lifestyle habits between the parents and their children. But an additional factor is likely to be model learning. James2 referred to the back seat of the family car as “road rage nursery”. Parents with children in the car have an opportunity of modeling good driving behavior by refraining from aggressive driving and making hostile commentaries about other drivers. The parent’s modeling of positive and supportive driving attitude can help adolescents identify with the moral dimension of driving behavior. This process of moral learning has been described by researchers as progressive in stages.

SOCIO-MORAL REASONING

Kohlberg analyzed the moral reasoning of the people of all ages and found that they differ in validity.4 Apparently, many adults have an underdeveloped moral sense and still use reasoning patterns that are more appropriate for children. Applying these notions to driving brings into focus that driving behavior has two components. One is cognitive and rational, and the other is affective or moral. For many drivers these two components seem to act independently. For instance, new drivers may be inexperienced and undeveloped cognitively, but morally they may be advanced, acting with prudence and decency. Many experienced drivers may have highly developed cognitive skills while their moral feelings remain underdeveloped. They drive with impulsiveness and aggression unconcerned about the increasing risks and potential injury to others. They drive as if their moral sense is not relevant to their driving.

According to Kelman5, external compliance is mediated by externally applied reinforcement such as reward for obedience and punishment for disobedience. For instance, drivers who regularly drive over the speed limit can get preoccupied with ‘watching out for cops’, or when a driver suspects a certain location to be a “speed trap”. Instances such as these indicate that compliance to speed limits is occasioned by external threat. This is identified as stage 1-compliance.

Stage 2-compliance involves operating with drivers who are motivated to look out for other motorists and road users. They are preoccupied with ‘watching out for others’ out of concern or a fear of injuring them. This level of moral operation could be called “identification” with other highway users. With such an altruistic motivation, the driver is more likely to stay within the speed limit out of sympathy for the safety of other highway users, as for example a driver who thinks, “I better not follow so close. Don’t want to intimidate that driver.” In other words, drivers in stage 2-compliance can be expected to agree with attitude items such as: other drivers have rights; we must all be fair to one another; objectivity in dealing with driving exchanges is safer and more pleasant in the long run; and so on.

Deeper levels of internalization are theoretically possible as described by Kohlberg4 on moral reasoning. Applying this approach to driving behavior, we can expect expressions of mutual concern, altruism, and religious values in connection with one’s driving experiences, as for example a driver who says, “I felt guilty for cutting in on that driver. They must have been really scared of not knowing whether I was going to hit them or not”, or “I keep thinking how closely I came to hit that man a while back. To think that I could be the cause of someone’s death or injury is really scary to me.”

CONCLUSION

A recent study, investigated the relationship between socio-moral reasoning and traffic safety among drivers in the Netherlands.14 The results showed that drivers in stage 1-compliance experienced a higher number of accidents, and drove faster and more aggressively. Interestingly, this relationship was more pronounced for men than women. It is clear that moral reasoning and socio-moral reflection ought to be present in driver education and public safety educational announcements. For example, public safety announcements on the radio routinely appeal for self-protection and financial cost as the motive for slowing down and driving cautiously. However, a few of the sports announcements make an appeal to the people’s sense of pro-social behavior and altruism by referring to the victims and their plight.

The moral dimension of driving has not yet become the focus of researchers and safety officials. Raising the moral thinking level of drivers in traffic may turn out to be an effective approach towards decreasing competitiveness and promoting cooperation with each other in the traffic.

1. Gulian E, Glendon AI, Matthews G, Davies DR, Debney LM. The stress of driving: A diary study. Work & Stress. 1990; 4(1): 7-16. doi: 10.1080/02678379008256960

2. James L. Aggressive driving and road rage–dealing with emotionally impaired drivers. Text of Congressional Testimony on Aggressive Drivers. Washington DC, USA: House Transportation Committee; 1997.

3. Hennessy DA, Wiesenthal DL, Kohn PM. The influence of traffic congestion, daily hassles, and trait stress susceptibility on state driver stress: An interactive perspective. J Appl Biobehav Res. 2000; 5(2): 162-179. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9861.2000.tb00072.x

4. Kohlberg L. Moral stages and moralization. In: Lickona T, ed. Moral Development and Behavior. New York, USA: Holt, Rinehart & Winston; 1976.

5. Kelman HC. Compliance, identification and internalization: Three processes of opinion change. J Conflict Resolut. 1958; 2(1): 51-60.

6. James L, Nahl D. Dealing with stress and pressure in the vehicle: Taxonomy of driving behavior: Affective, cognitive, sensorimotor. In: Rothe JP, ed. Driving Lessons – Exploring Systems that Make Traffic Safer. Edmonton, Canada: University of Alberta Press; 2002.

7. Sansone RA, Samsone LA. Road Rage: What’s Driving It? Psychiatry (Edgmont). 2010; 7(7): 14-18.

8. Rothe JP. Driven to Kill: Vehicles as Weapons. AB T6G 2E1, Canada: University of Alberta Press; 2008.

9. James L. The Effect of Age, Gender, and Type of Car Driven Across the States. Technical Report, Department of Psychology, Hawaii, USA: University of Hawaii; 2000.

10. Wickens CM, Wiesenthal DL, Roseborough JEW. Personality predictors of driver vengeance. Violence Vict. 2015; 30(1): 148-162. doi: 10.1891/0886-6708.VV-D-13-00111

11. James L, Nahl D. Road Rage and Aggressive Driving: Steering Clear of Highway Warfare. Amherst, New York, USA: Prometheus Books; 2000.

12. Bumgarner DJ, Webb JR, Dula CS. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour. Amsterdam, Netherlands: Elsevier; 2016: 317-331.

13. Bianchi A, Summala H. The “genetics” of driving behavior: Parents’ driving style predicts their children’s driving style. Accid Anal Prev. 2004; 36(4): 655-659.

14. Veldscholten N. Moral reasoning in Traffic: About the Possible Relations between Moral Reasoning and Traffic Safety. [master’s thesis]. Enschede, Netherlands: Educational Science and Technology; 2017.

LATEST ARTICLES

Chest X-ray Showed a Hazy Left Upper Lung Infiltrate

A Noteworthy Case of Myasthenic Crisis Induced by Levofloxacin

Ada Young*, Ramya Ramesh and Milind Awale

doi.

The Right Thigh Anterior Compartment was Swollen, and the Skin was Ulcerated due to the Traditional Cautery

Primary Skeletal Muscle Lymphoma: A Case Report and Literature Review

Solomon Bishaw*, Addisu Alemu and Abel Tefera

doi.

An Unusual Presentation of Encephalitis in a Patient with Lyme Neuroborreliosis

Maithily Patel*, Jazmin Jatana, Ramya Ramesh and Milind Awale

doi.

Practical Pointers for Drug Development and Medical Affairs

Gerald L. Klein*, Roger E. Morgan, Shabnam Vaezzadeh, Burak Pakkal and Pavle Vukojevic

doi.

10.17140/CTPOJ-7-125

Prevalence and Risk Factors of Subclinical Mastitis of Goats in Banadir Region, Somalia

Omar M. Salah*, Yasin H. Sh-Hassan, Moktar O. S. Mohamed, Mohamed A. Yusuf and Abas S. A. Jimale

doi.10.17140/VMOJ-9-184

LATEST ARTICLES