Psychology and Cognitive Sciences

Open journal

ISSN 2380-727X

Induced Boredom Constrains Mindfulness: An Online Demonstration

Samuel R Koval and McWelling Todman*

McWelling Todman, PhD, Associate Professor for Clinical Practice, Department of Psychology, The New School for Social Research, 80 Fifth Ave, New York 10011, USA. E-mail: todmanm@newschool.edu

INTRODUCTION

The concept of mindfulness was codified in the 3rd century BC by Buddhist scholars and meditation teachers. These Theravada Abbhidamma texts define it as, “presence of mind, attentiveness to the present.”2 More contemporary attempts to characterize mindfulness have provided somewhat more elaborate descriptions but ultimately differ more in terms of emphasis than on substance. For example, Lazar describes mindfulness as a meditative state in which there is an active “exploration of the distractions to concentration, such as sensations, thoughts and feelings,”3 a definition that is not too different from those4 who describe mindfulness as open, receptive, undivided awareness and attention to internal and external experience in the present moment.

Minor definitional differences notwithstanding, there is now a substantial body of evidence to suggest that mindfulness is associated with a host of beneficial physical and psychological effects, including measurable reductions in levels of anxiety, depression and even subjective pain.5 As a consequence, there has been a corresponding interest among clinicians and researchers in the therapeutic potential of mindfulness in the promotion of psychological and physical well-being.

Interest in boredom has also increased over the last several years, especially with respect to its growing list of associated liabilities. Indeed, it has not gone unnoticed that boredom, both a trait and state, appears to be the antithesis of mindfulness. Whereas, mindfulness is known to promote subjective wellbeing and to mitigate a number of negative states, boredom and the propensity to become bored appear to promote the opposite. Moreover, in contrast to the definition of mindfulness, many of the definitions of boredom describe a state in which distractions to concentration are not only unrecognized, they are in fact deliberately and effortfully resisted at the cost of increasing levels of subjective distress.6,7,8,9

Despite this apparent inverted relationship between the conceptual and empirical corollaries of mindfulness and boredom there has been a surprising lack of interest in empirically examining the presumed relationship between the two. This is unfortunate, especially since the capacity for mindfulness through training and practice can be enhanced in most individuals, thus raising the possibility that mindfulness training might also increase the capacity to cope with boredom and by extension forestall some of its attendant negative consequences. The current study was an attempt to address this oversight using a web-based platform to examine the relationship between state mindfulness and induced boredom in an online sample of participants. Importantly, we are unaware of any reports in the literature of previous attempts to induce boredom in participants in an online study. Thus an additional and secondary goal of the study was to examine the utility and effectiveness of a novel and potentially far more efficient approach to the induction of boredom states.

CORRELATES OF BOREDOM

Though long ignored, research of the last twenty years suggests that boredom and the propensity to become bored are associated with a number of social and psychological problems. For example, boredom proneness has been positively associated with measures of hopelessness,10 loneliness,10 hostility and anger,11 anxiety,12 somatization complaints13 decreased sexual satisfaction14 poorly developed interpersonal relationships,15 impulsiveness,16 lowered motivational orientation,10 lowered academic achievement,17,18,19,20 poor performance in the workplace,21,22 job dissatisfaction21,22 increased levels of alcohol abuse,23,24 overeating,25 pathological gambling,26 drug use,v psychotic symptoms,7,8,9 physical symptoms,11 and depression.10,12,27

Less clear, however, is the reason why boredom has proven to be such a powerful predictor of psychosocial dysfunction. One plausible explanation that has been frequently put forward is that attentional failure is a defining and necessary feature of the experience of boredom,28,29,30,31 and it is this loss of attentional control that is at the core of many of the problems that have been found to be associated with boredom prone individuals.

ATTENTIONAL THEORIES OF BOREDOM

Attentional theories of boredom suggest that a disruption of attentional regulation is at the root of the experience of boredom.28,29,30,31 Eastwood et al. for example, have proposed that boredom depends on three conditions: (1) An inability to successfully engage attention with internal or external environments in a sufficiently stimulating way; (2) awareness of that failure of attention, and; (3) the attribution of the aversive experience to the external environment. Todman7,9 has also emphasized the important role of attentional dyscontrol, but has argued that a more precise characterization must give prominence to the feelings of attentional constraint that are invariably experienced when the impulse to shift attention is persistently frustrated by an external or internal injunction that prohibits the desired shift.

It is also seems that the attentional options matter when trying to cope with boredom. Even when the individual does not feel an obligation to remain attentive to an uninteresting task or stimulus, feelings of attentional constraint might obtain if the alternative attentional targets available to the individual are experienced as unappealing or unrewarding. In a compelling demonstration of this point, Critcher and Gilovich32 conducted a series of studies in which they manipulated the content of the mind-wanderings of the study participants and found that they were significantly more likely to report boredom with an ongoing task when their daydreams were about negative events, than when their minds wandered to positive or rewarding narratives. This finding underscores not only the importance of the broadened notion of attentional constraint but also the contention that boredom is an evolutionarily prepared signal that indicates that valuable attentional resources are being squandered on an activity or environment in which the potential for positive reinforcement has been depleted below a certain threshold.7,9 In short, for the feelings of boredom to be reduced or avoided, it is necessary that the shift in attention be directed to an alternative source of reinforcement that is construed as being potentially more positive in nature; something that can be accomplished by one of two strategies: (1) Changing the actual environment to one that is richer in potential positive reinforcement or; (2) Engaging the same environment differently in order to expose hitherto undiscovered sources of reinforcement. If this conjecture is accurate, then it makes sense that an experiential strategy that is described as being a mode of positively engaging the environment (i.e., mindfulness) is probably a desirable resource to have at ones disposal.

MINDFULNESS AND BOREDOM

Although the need for further research on the apparent association between the constructs of boredom and mindfulness has been proposed by a number of researchers,33,34 we have been able to identify only two such studies in a review of the recent literature. In the earlier of the two studies, Trunnell, et al35 measured the effects of a mindfulness training class for college students registered for recreation and leisure classes such as kayaking, camping, and back packing skills. The authors reported finding a greater decrease in boredom in the experimental group, which received a 10-minute mindfulness didactic, followed by 15-minutes of guided meditation, than in the control group, which received no mindfulness training. However, the authors did not measure boredom directly. Instead, they inferred its existence on the basis of a discriminant function analysis of the participants’ responses on a modified version of Russell and McAuley’s Causal Dimension Scale,36 which contained a list 14 positive and 14 negative affects but made no reference to boredom. The authors conjectured that the constellation of affects contained in the predictive function could be reasonably construed as boredom.

In the second and more recent study, using two psychometrically valid measures-the Mindfulness Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS)4 and the Boredom Proneness Scale (BPS)37,  LePera found that the traits of boredom proneness (the propensity to become bored) and mindfulness are negatively correlated38. However, the study focused exclusively on traits, rather than the actual states of boredom and mindfulness. Thus it remains unclear whether feelings of boredom can actually be mitigated by a state of mindfulness and vice versa. In order to more directly address this relationship, the present study examined the question of whether individuals subjected to a boring task would show lower state mindfulness scores compared to individuals in a control task.

METHODS

Participants

One hundred and sixteen participants were recruited from Amazon Mechanical Turk, a web based recruitment interface, to participate in “online research on the influence of different types of visual stimuli on learning styles.” Participants were compensated with a payment of 60 cents. While Mechanical Turk is largely considered to be a reliable and, in many ways, an advantageous sorce of data for social science research, precautionary exclusion criteria have been recomendend to ensure the quality of data.39 In the present study, data was excluded from analyses if the participant finished the study unusually quickly (time<20 mins, n=1), took an unusually long amount of time to complete the study (time>80 mins, n=3), or failed to correctly answer any of the four validation items[1] that were hidden among the scales and demographic questionnaire (validation score<26, n=17).

After these exclusions (n=21), ninety-five participants (53 female, 42 male) were included in the analyses. The mean age was 38.9 years (SD=12.62). The majority of the participants were “Non-Hispanic White/Euro American” (44%, n=42), “Asian/Asian American or Pacific Islander” (41%, n=39), and “Black/African-American” (5%, n=4). Seventy-two percent were employed (n=68), and seventy-nine percent had attained an associates degree or higher (n=69). See Table 1, for a detailed summary of the sample characterstics.

 

Table 1: Sample characteristics
Characteristic N (%) Mean (SD)
Age 38.89 (12.62)
Gender
Male 42(44.2)
Female 53(55.8)
Ethnicity/Race

Non-Hispanic White/Euro-American/

Caucasian

42(44.2)

Black/African-American/Afro Carib

bean

4(4.2)
Asian/Asian-American or Pacific
Islander
39(41.1)

Latino/a, Hispanic-American,

Chicano/a

2(2.1)
Native American/Alaskan Native 1(1.1)
Middle Eastern/Arab American 2(2.1)
Multi-Racial/Multi Ethnic 2(2.1)
Other/ Prefer not to say 3(3.2)
Marital Status
Single/dating casually 30(31)
Dating seriously/not living together 1(1)
Living with partner but not married 10(11)
Married or in marriage relationship 54(57)
Number of children 1.02(1.25)
Sexual Orientation
Heterosexual 85(90)
Gay/Lesbian 3(3)
Bisexual 2(2)
Queer 1(1)
Transgender 4(4)
Highest Level of Education

High school diploma/GED

BA/BS

9(10)

22(67)

Some college 17(18)
Associates degree 7(7)
BA/BS 32(34)
Some graduate school 3(3)
MA/MBA/JD 22(23)
PhD/MD 5 (5)
Are you currently a student?
         No 81(85.3)
         Part-time 8(8.4)
         Full-time 6(6.3)
Annual Household Income
         $0 – $20,000 25(26.3)
         $20,000 – $30,000 19(20)
         $30,000 – $50,000 20(21.1)
         $50,000 – $100,000 18(18.9)
         $100,000 – $150,000 9(9.5)
         50,000 – $200,000 1(1)
         Prefer not to say 3(3.2)
Country of Origin
         USA 41(43.2)
   India 34(35.8)
UK 3(3.2)
Canada 3(3.2)
Ethiopia 1(1.1)
Jordan 1(1.1)
Peru 1(1.1)
South Korea 1(1.1)
Brazil 1(1.1)
Vietnam 1(1.1)
Trinidad and Tobago 1(1.1)
Primary/Native Language
         English 59(62.1)
   Other than English 18(18.9)
   Bi-lingual 18(18.9)
How serious are you about faith/spirituality?
        Not at all 16(16.8)
        Somewhat 26(27.4)
        Quite 22(23.2)
        Very 31(32.6)
Religious Tradition
        Christian 26(27.4)
        Catholic 12(12.6)
        Protestant 3(3.2)
        Mormon/Latter Day Saints 1(1.1)
        Muslim 3(3.2)
        Hindu 20(21.1)
        Agnostic 6(6.3)
      Atheist 11(11.6)
      Spiritual 5(5.3)
      Other 7(7.4)
      No response 18(55)
Do you meditate or practice mindfulness?
      No/never 32(33.7)
      I’ve tried it and liked it 12(12.6)
      I’ve tried it and didn’t like it 6 (6.3)
      I practice sometimes 26(27.4)
      I practice about once a week or more 9 (9.5)
      I practice every day or nearly every day 10(10.5)
Political Orientation
      Liberal 32(33.7)
      Moderate 46(48.6)
      Conservative 17(17.9)
Current Health Status
      Excellent 21(22.1)
      Good 56(58.9)
      Fair 17(17.9)
      Poor    1(1.1)
In my child hood I grew up with
      Two biological parents 76 (80)
      A single biological parent 8 (8.4)

      One biological parent and one step parent

6 (6.3)

      In shared custody between two parents

2 (2.1)
      Two adoptive parents 2 (2.1)
      Under the care of relatives 1 (1.1)
Culture values communicating emotions?
      Yes 50 (52.6)
      No 30 (31.6)
      I don’t know 15 (15.8)
Currently live in
      My home city 32 (33.7)
      My home state 25 (26.3)
      My home country 33 (34.7)
      Not my home country 5 (5.3)

 

Because Mechanical Turk is a web-based platform, the sample constituted a multinational population, with 50 percent living outside the United States (n=48), and most of those individuals residing in India (36%, n=34). Also of note, thirty-two percent reported that they practiced mindfulness-like activities “sometimes,” “once a week,” or “almost every day.”

MATERIALS

The Vowel Cancellation Task (VCT) was used to induce boredom. The VCT, which has been used extensively in the laboratory of the second author to rapidly induce feelings of boredom, is conceptually similar to vigilance tasks that require the participants to maintain attention on uninteresting stimuli for a sustained period of time. On the VCT, participants are presented with a moderately interesting short story2 about 20 pages in length. The story is divided into 16 roughly equal sections and each section is followed by a text box in which participants are instructed to record the number of vowels counted (i.e., cancelled) in that section. The task lasts 15 minutes after which a researcher notifies the subject that the task is over. When the task is presented on a computer, as was the case in the present study, the task ends automatically after 15 minutes. Because the current study utilized a computer-based presentation mode, an “incorrect total” warning was added to the VCT protocol, which appeared whenever a participant entered an erroneous vowel count, something that is not possible with the paper and pencil version of the VCT. It was thought that this addendum would increase attendance to the already boring task and therefore increase boredom even further. Participants in the control condition simply read the short story for 15 minutes.

As a manipulation check participants were asked to indicate on a likert-type scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 5 (extremely) the degree to which they would describe the task that they performed (i.e., reading or vowel cancelation) as being anxiety-provoking, amusing, boring, tedious, or enjoyable, and also the degree to which they currently felt anxious, amused, bored, annoyed or joyful. The expectation was that in comparison to participants in the control condition, participants in the vowel cancelation condition would be more likely to describe the task as being boring and/or tedious and to describe themselves as being bored.


Footnote

 a.[1]The validations items included prompts such as “are you using the internet right now?” and “what is 3+3?” which were scaled in such a way as to blend in with the items around them. A validation composite score was based on the four items with a maximun of 28 and a minimum of 4.


 

The State Mindfulness Scale (SMS):40 was used in the present study to assess state mindfulness. It operationalizes mindfulness defined as a meta cognitive state that is characterized by what one is paying attention to (body sensations and mental events occurring in the present moment) and how one is paying attention (deliberately in the present, with awareness, sensitivity, intimacy with subjective experience, and curiosity). The scale consists of 21 items to which respondents indicate how well each statement describes what they just experienced by utilizing a likert-type scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 5 (very well). Total scores range from 21 to 105 with higher scores indicating a greater degree of state mindfulness. The internal consistency coefficient reported was alpha=.95.

The Boredom Proneness Scale (BPS):10 is a 28-item questionnaire that measures an individual’s susceptibility to the experience of boredom. Sample items include “Much of the time I just sit around doing nothing,” and “When I was young, I was often in monotonous and tire some situations.” Responses are scored on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from “strongly disagree” (1) to “strongly agree” (7). Responses are summed to form a boredom proneness score ranging from 28 to 196, with higher scores indicating a higher susceptibility to the experience of boredom. The BPS is considered to be a trait measure and to be psychometrically sound (alpha=.79, test-retest correlation=.83).

The Multidimensional State Boredom Scale (MSBS):1 is a 29-item questionnaire that measures the current experience of boredom. Sample items include “Time is moving very slowly” and “I seem to be forced to do things that have no value to me.” Responses are scored on a 7-point Likert like scale ranging from “strongly disagree” (1) to “strongly agree” (7). The MSBS assesses boredom across five dimensions (i.e. attention, disengagement, agitation, dysphoria, and sense of time passing). Responses are summed to produce a total score ranging from 29 to 203 with higher scores indicating a higher level of state boredom. The scale is internally consistent, alpha= .95. The internal consistency of each factor ranges from .80 to .92.

Demographics Questionnaire: A demographics questionnaire was administered, which asked about age, sex, race/ethnicity, relationship status, sexual orientation, gender identity, education, country of origin, religious identification, and other common demographic categories. Also included was an item, “Do you meditate or practice mindfulness?” This item was included because of the likelihood that meditation experience might affect response patterns to the primary measures. For example, the validity of the experiment would be strengthened if meditation exposure were positively related to state mindfulness. Response options were, “No/never,” “I’ve tried it and liked it,” I’ve tried it and didn’t like it,” “I practice sometimes,” I practice about once a week or more,” “I practice every day or nearly every day.”

PROCEDURE

The posting on Amazon Mechanical Turk included a link to the study, which was built using Qualtrics software for web-based data collection. After formally consenting to participate in the study, participants were randomly assigned, using the Qualtrics randomization feature, to either the Vowel Cancellation Task (n=46) or the Reading Task (n=49). Based on pilot data, which suggested that the drop-out rate would be substantially higher during the VCT than during the less aversive Reading Task, the randomization feature was set to assign participants to the VCT at a rate roughly twice that of the rate of assignment to the control condition, thus ensuring a relatively equal number of study completers in both conditions.

In both conditions, participants were instructed to set aside distractions, give their full attention to the focal task, and try to be as accurate as possible while working quickly. The duration of the task was not specified, which likely accounts for drop-out rates across both conditions, as most dropouts occurred during this task phase.

After 15 minutes of working on the assigned task, all participants were automatically advanced to the questionnaire section of the study. The manipulation check was administered immediately after completion of the induction or reading task. Next, the MSBS and the SMS were administered and the order of presentation was counterbalanced, followed by the BPS and the demographics questionnaire. Lastly, participants were debriefed and the purpose of the study explained. Compensation was delayed because each participant’s work had to be formally “approved” within the Mechanical Turk system but for no more than 72 hours.

RESULTS

The SMS, MSBS and BPS all showed good to excellent internal consistency (alpha= .81-.96) and each alpha was within 2 hundredths of a point of the respective scale’s published alpha coefficients. Despite the small sample, the within group distributions for the SMS, MSBS, and BPS did not violate the assumption of normality, even when the data was factored by gender. Independent sample T-tests confirmed that neither boredom proneness nor any demographic variables significantly differed across conditions.

A comparison of the mean scores of the experimental and control group on each item of the manipulation check confirmed that participants in the vowel cancellation condition were significantly more annoyed and enjoyed themselves less. Moreover, participants in the vowel cancellation condition rated the vowel counting task as significantly more anxiety-provoking, more boring, more tedious and less enjoyable, compared to the control group’s ratings of the reading task. In other words, the boredom task accomplished what it was intended to do; which was to affect an aversive state characterized by feelings of boredom, tedium and annoyance attributed to the focal task (Table 2).

 

Table 2: Means and Standard Deviations for Manipulation Check items by Experimental Condition.
Condition
Items Vowel Cancellation Task (boredom) Reading Task (control) t
Mean SD N Mean SD N
I am anxious 3.07 1.25 46 2.61 1.22 49 ns
I am amused 2.09 1.28 46 2.24 1.28 49 ns
I am bored 2.30 1.36 46 1.82 1.17 49 ns
I am enjoying myself 2.41 1.39 46 3.06 1.07 49 -2.53*
I am annoyed 2.89 1.35 46 1.96 1.22 49 3.52***
The task is anxiety-provoking 3.22 1.21 46 2.71 1.14 49 2.09*
The task is amusing 2.04 1.26 46 2.20 1.29 49 ns
The task is boring 2.65 1.42 46 1.78 1.23 49 3.21*
The task is enjoyable 2.26 1.41 46 3.08 1.15 49 -3.12*
The task is tedious 3.65 1.43 46 2.35 1.18 49 4.85***
Note. * = p< .05, *** = p < .001.

 

As predicted, participants in the boredom condition (M=105.7, SD=32.3) also scored significantly higher on the MSBS than their counterparts in the control condition (M=90.4, SD=32.9), t (93)=2.295, p= .024, and dropped out at significantly higher rates (roughly 45% more frequently) than participants in the control condition (Tables 3 and 4). Together with the manipulation check results, these findings are similar to the results that have been observed with the VCT in laboratory settings and suggest that with the proper safeguards the online version of the VCT might be viable option for studies involving boredom induction.

 

Table 3: Means and Standard deviations for the Multidimensional State Boredom Scale and the State Mindfulness Scale by Experimental Condition.
Condition
Items Boredom Induction Task Reading Task (control) t
Mean SD N Mean SD N
MSBS 105.74 32.29 46 90.39 32.86 49 2.30*
SMS 64.02 21.18 46 72.02 16.96 49 2.02*
Note. * = p< .05, *** = p < .001.

 

Table 4: Participants Recruited, Drop-out Rates and Exclusions Due to Time or Reliability by Experimental Condition.
Condition
Vowel Cancellation Task (boredom) Reading Task (control) Total
Total Recruited 268 132 400
Drop-out1 212 72 284
Drop-out rate 79.1% 54.5% 71%
Excluded due to time or reliability2 10 11 21
Sample used for analysis 46 49 95
1Dropout refers to participants who discontinued participation before completing the reading task or the vowel counting exercise.
2Participants were excluded if they took longer than 80 minutes or less than 20 minutes to complete the survey, or it they failed to correctly answer more than one of four validation items (“What does 3+3 = ?”).

 

T-tests were used to analyze group differences with regard to state mindfulness. Effect size is reported with Cohen’s d. Compared with controls participants in the boredom condition (M=64.02, SD=21.18) scored significantly lower on the SMS than their counterparts (M=72.02, SD=16.96), supporting the main hypothesis that state boredom constrains the experience of state mindfulness, t (86.119) = -2.024, p= .044, d= .418 (Table 3).

In the sample as a whole, and also consistent with the study predictions, self-reported meditation exposure was found to be correlated with SMS scores, r (93) = .271, p = .008. Moreover, participants who reported that they practiced meditation “weekly” or “almost every day” displayed higher SMS scores (M=73.30, SD=17.06) compared those who practiced “sometimes,” had only “tried” meditation, or had never practiced (M= 63.30, SD= 20.32), t (93) = 2.643, p= .010, d= .543. Also, the BPS was correlated with the MSBS, r (74)= .683, p<.001, a finding that is in line with the results that have been reported in the construct validation studies of the MSBS.1

SUMMARY AND DISCUSSION

As predicted, the boredom-induction manipulation had a moderate but significant effect on self-reported state mindfulness, thus supporting the central hypothesis of the study, which is that state boredom constrains the capacity to experience mindfulness. This finding also extends the findings from the earlier study,36 which focused exclusively on the trait forms of boredom (boredom proneness) and mindfulness. By demonstrating that the state forms of the two constructs have a similarly inverse relationship, the present study provides further support for the widely held notion that one of the important potential benefits of mindfulness training is an enhancement in the capacity to cope with boredom. It is also consistent with the growing evidence that mindfulness-based interventions may be particularly effective in high risk populations in which boredom, distractibility and impulsiveness are especially prominent – the most obvious example being individuals diagnosed with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD).41,42

Basic support for the effectiveness of the electronic online version of Vowel Cancellation Task was provided by the manipulation check items, the MSBS, which showed a significant elevation in boredom following the vowel cancellation task, and the differential drop-out rates across groups. However, replication is needed before any firm conclusions can be made about the soundness of the approach, despite its apparent promise.

The differential drop-out rates across conditions is consistent with previous research that supports the association of boredom with decreased vigilance and sustained attention,43,28 and is also consistent with the conceptualization of boredom as a cue to reallocate attentional resources toward more rewarding activity.8 Apparently, in this case, the VCT was experienced as being boring enough to send substantially more participants in search of something else to do, while the participants in the Reading Task were more likely to maintain their interest.

One questions that remains is whether participants in the boredom condition were more likely to drop out because they were initially or temperamentally more mindful, thus biasing the results. Unfortunately, however, due to the constraints of the study design it was not possible address this question by directly comparing the levels of mindfulness among completers vs. dropouts. In addition to the fact that the study did not include a measure of trait mindfulness, all of the dropouts in the current study occurred during the VCT or reading task, and thus before the administration of the outcome measures. However, there are good reasons to believe that it is unlikely that the results are attributable to higher rates of attrition among individuals with higher mindfulness scores. For example, findings from earlier research on the impact of mindfulness on the capacity to sustain attention and persist on aversive tasks suggests that mindful participants would be expected to be less, not more, likely to dropout from tasks like the VCT.44 Furthermore, as previously mentioned, Lepera38  was able to show in her study that individuals with higher levels of trait mindfulness tend to be less susceptible to boredom, and thus less likely to discontinue intrinsically boring tasks like the VCT.

Finally, although the present study establishes support for state boredom’s dampening effect on the experience of mindfulness, it does not address the more clinically relevant question of whether induced mindfulness inoculates against or mitigates feelings of boredom. Clearly this is a direction that future studies should pursue. For example, previous studies interested in the effects of mindfulness have induced mindfulness using a 15-minute guided meditation.45 Utilizing such a methodology would not only address the important question of whether the relationship between states of boredom and mindfulness are truly bidirectional and symmetrical, but also whether it is possible to deliver mindfulness training in an online format.

1. Fahlman SA, Mercer-Lynn KB, Flora DB, Eastwood JD. Development and validation of the multidimensional state boredom scale. Assessment. 2013; 20: 68-85. doi: 10.1177/1073191111421303

2. Bohdi B. A Comprehensive Manual of Abhidhamma: The AbhidhammatthaSangaha (Vipassana Meditation and the Buddha’s Teachings) Sri Lanka: Pariyatti Publishing; Buddhist Publication Society. 2012; 86.

3. Lazar SW. Mindfulness and Psychotherapy. In: Germer Ck, Siegel RD, Fulton PR. Mindfulness Research. 1st ed. Newyork, NY, USA: Guilford Press; 2005; 220.

4. Brown KW, Ryan RM. Perils and promise in defining and measuring mindfulness: Observations from experience. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice. 2004; 11(3): 242-248. doi: 10.1093/clipsy.bph078

5. Kabat-Zinn J, Hanh TN. Full catastrophe living: Using the wisdom of your body and mind to face stress, pain, and illness. Random House LLC. 2009.

6. Mikulas WL, Vodanovich SJ. The essence of boredom. The Psychological Record. 1993.

7. Todman M. Boredom in Schizoaffective Disorder and other Psychotic Conditions. In: Yip K. Schizoaffective Disordesr: International Perspectives on Understanding, Intervention and Rehabilitation. New York: Nova Science Publications; 2003.

8. Todman M. Psychopathology and Boredom: A Neglected Association. In: Fanti KA. Psychological Science: Research, Theory and Future Directions. Athens: ATINER press; 2007.

9. Todman M. The dimensions of state boredom: Frequency, duration, unpleasantness, consequences and causal attributions. Educational Research International. 2013; 1(1): 32-40.

10. Farmer R, Sundberg ND. Boredom proneness–the development and correlates of a new scale. Journal of personality assessment. 1986; 50(1): 4-17. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa5001_2

11. Rupp DE, Vodanovich SJ. The role of boredom proneness in self-reported anger and aggression. Journal of Social Behaviour and Personality. 1997; 12(4): 925-936.

12. Gordon A, Wilkinson R, McGown, A, Jovanoska S. The psychometric properties of the Boredom Proneness Scale: An examination of its validity. Psychological Studies. 1997; 42: 85-97.

13. Sommers J, Vodanovich SJ. Boredom proneness: Its relationship to psychological-and physical-health symptoms. Journal of clinical psychology. 2000; 56(1): 149-155. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-4679(200001)56:1<149::AID-JCLP14>3.0.CO;2-Y

14. Watt JD, Ewing JE. Toward the development and validation of a measure of sexual boredom. The Journal of Sex Research. 1996; 33: 57-66. doi: 10.1080/00224499609551815

15. Watt JD, Vodanovich SJ. Boredom proneness and psychosocial development. The Journal of psychology. 1999; 133(3): 303-314. doi: 10.1080/00223989909599743

16. Watt JD, Vodanovich SJ. Relationship between boredom proneness and impulsivity. Psychological reports. 1992; 70(3): 688-690. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1992.70.3.688

17. Lehr E, Todman M. Boredom and Boredom Proneness in Children: Implications for Academic and Social Adjustment. In: Todman M. Self-Regulation and Social Competence: Psychological Studies in Identity, Achievement and Work-Family Dynamics. Athens: ATNIER Press; 2009; 79-90.

18. Maroldo GK. Shyness, boredom, and grade point average among college students. Psychological Reports. 1986; 59(2): 395-398. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1986.59.2.395

19. Paulson MJ, Coombs RH, Richardson MA. School performance, academic aspirations, and drug use among children and adolescents. Journal of drug education. 1990; 20(4): 289-303. doi: 10.2190/8j0x-ly6d-pl7w-42fa

20. Robinson WP. Boredom at school. British Journal of Educational Psychology. 1975; 45(2): 141-152. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8279.1975.tb03239.x

21. Fisher CD. Boredom at work: A neglected concept. Human Relations. 1993; 46(3): 395-417. doi: 10.1177/001872679304600305

22. O’Hanlon JF. Boredom: Practical consequences and a theory. Actapsychologica. 1981; 49(1): 53-82. doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(81)90033-0

23. Krotava I, Todman M. Boredom Severity, Depression and Alcohol Consumption in Belarus. Journal of Psychology and Behavioral Science. 2014; 2(1): 73-83.

24. Patrick ME, Schulenberg JE. How trajectories of reasons for alcohol use relate to trajectories of binge drinking: National panel data spanning late adolescence to early adulthood. Developmental psychology. 2011; 47(2): 311. doi: 10.1037/a0021939

25. Wilson GD. Eating style, obesity and health. Personality and Individual Differences. 1986; 7(2): 215-224. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(86)90058-9

26. Blaszczynski A, McConaghy N, Frankova A. Boredom proneness in pathological gambling. Psychological Reports. 1990; 67(1): 35-42. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1990.67.1.35

27. Vodanovich SJ. Psychometric measures of boredom: A review of the literature. The Journal of psychology. 2003; 137(6): 569-595. doi: 10.1080/00223980309600636

28. Eastwood JD, Frischen A, Fenske MJ, Smilek D. The Unengaged Mind Defining Boredom in Terms of Attention. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2012; 7(5): 482-495. doi 10.1177/1745691612456044

29. Fisher CD. Boredom at work: A neglected concept. Human Relations. 1993; 46(3): 395-417. doi: 10.1177/001872679304600305

30. Hamilton JA. Attention, personality, and the self-regulation of mood: Absorbing interest and boredom. Progress in experimental personality research. 1981; 10(28): 1-315.

31. Harris MB. Correlates and Characteristics of Boredom Proneness and Boredom. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 2000; 30(3): 576-598. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2000.tb02497.x

32. Critcher CR, Gilovich T. Inferring attitudes from mind wandering. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2010; 36(9): 1255-1266. doi: 10.1177/0146167210375434

33. Martin M, Sadlo G, Stew G. The phenomenon of boredom. Qualitative Research in Psychology. 2006; 3(3): 193-211. doi: 10.1191/1478088706qrp066oa

34. Baer RA, Fischer S, Huss DB. Mindfulness and acceptance in the treatment of disordered eating. Journal of rational-emotive and cognitive-behavior therapy. 2005; 23(4): 281-300. doi: 10.1007/s10942-005-0015-9

35. Trunnell EP, White F, Cederquist J, Braza J. Optimizing an Outdoor Experience for Experiential Learning by Decreasing Boredom through Mindfulness Training. Journal of Experiential Education. 1996; 19(1): 43-49. doi: 10.1177/105382599601900109

36. Russell D, McAuley E. Causal attributions, causal dimensions, and affective reactions to success and failure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1986; 50: 1174-1185. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.50.6.1174

37. Farmer R, Sundberg ND. Boredom proneness–the development and correlates of a new scale. Journal of personality assessment. 1986; 50(1): 4-17. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa5001_2

38. LePera N. The relationships between boredom proneness, mindfulness, anxiety, depression, and substance use. The New School Psychology Bulletin. 2011; 8(2).

39. Buhrmester M, Kwang T, Gosling SD. Amazon’s Mechanical Turk a new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data?. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2011; 6(1): 3-5. doi: 10.1177/1745691610393980

40. Tanay G, Bernstein A. State Mindfulness Scale (SMS): Development and initial validation. Psychological assessment. 2013; 25(4): 1286. doi: 10.1037/a0034044

41. Edel M, Holter, T, Wassink K, Juckel G.   A comparision of mindfulness-based group training and skills group training in adults. Journal of Attention Disorders (online pre-publication edition). 2014. doi: 10.1177/1087054714551635

42. Zylowska L, Ackerman D, Yang M, et al. Mindfulness meditation training in adults and adolescents with ADHD: A feasibility study. Journal of Attention Disorders. 2007; 11(6): 737-774. doi: 10.1177/1087054707308502

43. Thomson DR, Besner D, Smilek D. A Resource-Control Account of Sustained Attention Evidence From Mind-Wandering and Vigilance Paradigms. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2015; 10(1): 82-96. doi: 10.1177/1745691614556681

44. Semple RJ. Does mindfulness meditation enhance attention? A randomized controlled trial. Mindfulness. 2010; 1(2): 121-130. doi: 10.1007/s12671-010-0017-2

45. Lau MA, Bishop SR, Segal ZV, et al. The Toronto mindfulness scale: Development and validation. Journal of clinical psychology. 2006; 62(12): 1445-1467. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20326

LATEST ARTICLES

The Right Thigh Anterior Compartment was Swollen, and the Skin was Ulcerated due to the Traditional Cautery

Primary Skeletal Muscle Lymphoma: A Case Report and Literature Review

Solomon Bishaw*, Addisu Alemu and Abel Tefera

doi.

An Unusual Presentation of Encephalitis in a Patient with Lyme Neuroborreliosis

Maithily Patel*, Jazmin Jatana, Ramya Ramesh and Milind Awale

doi.

Practical Pointers for Drug Development and Medical Affairs

Gerald L. Klein*, Roger E. Morgan, Shabnam Vaezzadeh, Burak Pakkal and Pavle Vukojevic

doi.

10.17140/CTPOJ-7-125

Prevalence and Risk Factors of Subclinical Mastitis of Goats in Banadir Region, Somalia

Omar M. Salah*, Yasin H. Sh-Hassan, Moktar O. S. Mohamed, Mohamed A. Yusuf and Abas S. A. Jimale

doi.10.17140/VMOJ-9-184

Use of Black Soldier Fly (Hermetia illucens) Prepupae Reared on Organic Waste

Maggot Debridement Therapy: A Natural Solution for Wound Healing

Isayas A. Kebede*, Haben F. Gebremeskel and Gelan D. Dahesa,

doi.10.17140/VMOJ-9-183

LATEST ARTICLES

The Right Thigh Anterior Compartment was Swollen, and the Skin was Ulcerated due to the Traditional Cautery

Case Report

2024 Apr

Solomon Bishaw*, Addisu Alemu and Abel Tefera

Case Report

2024 Apr

Maithily Patel*, Jazmin Jatana, Ramya Ramesh and Milind Awale