Psychology and Cognitive Sciences

Open journal

ISSN 2380-727X

A Randomized Controlled Study on the Effects of a Documentary on Students’ Empathy and Attitudes towards Older Adults

Luciana Laganá*, Larisa Gavrilova, Delwin B. Carter and Andrew T. Ainsworth

Luciana Laganá, PhD

Professor, Psychology Department, California State University Northridge Northridge, CA 91330, USA Tel. 818-677-2827 E-mail: luciana.lagana@csun.edu

INTRODUCTION

With populations aging rapidly, ageism is becoming a pronounced social issue that will affect societies throughout the world in the coming decades. Although aging as an individual is an inevitable biological process for every living person, our society holds negative stereotypes and prejudices towards older adults in the form of ageism.1,2 According to Butler, ageism is ‘‘a process of systematic stereotyping and discrimination against people because they are old’’.3 Even though prejudice and discrimination come in many forms, ageism has not been given as much research attention as it deserves. Compared to sexism or racism, for example, research on negative attitudes towards older populations is scant.4-5 Yet, ageism may be the most commonly held form of negative stereotype and the most experienced form of discrimination.6

The growing profusion of age-related stereotypes and discrimination is driven, at least in part, by various media outlets (e.g., television, movies, magazines, and social media). While media has the potential to positively influence people to be more understanding and less discriminatory towards older adults, the opposite often occurs. As society places greater importance on physical appearance, beauty and youth can easily be idolized, while aging is typically associated with physical and cognitive impairments and dependence.7-8 In this regard, higher levels of internalization of North American appearance ideals among undergraduate students have been associated with an expression of more negative attitudes towards older adults.9 A striking similarity exists between ageism and the discrimination of disabled people, as older adults are often perceived as physically incapable and sick.10-11 Additionally, the media sends frequent messages to young individuals that older people are an economic burden to society,12 further augmenting the divide between younger and older adults.

The aforementioned age-related divide has also been linked to lowered feelings of empathy towards older adults in society.13 Empathy has been classically defined as an emotional response to someone else’s situation or emotional state.14 When feeling empathy, an individual experiences someone else’s affective responses.15 Ageism is a form of prejudice that many people hold towards their own potential future selves5,16 as a method to reduce their anxiety related to aging and death; thus, it often leads to individuals categorizing older adults under an “other” group or out-group in which the younger individual does not belong.17 Placing older adults into an out-group helps people a) distance themselves from older adults and b) externalize the impact of aging. This distancing process can be linked to the individuals’ efforts to diminish the anxiety that they feel towards their own future1,5 and, consequently, could diminish interpersonal empathy towards older adults as well as increase ageist attitudes.

The spread of ageist views and discriminatory behavior against older adults has the potential to impact everyone in the society, even the very professionals who are supposed to care for older adults. Researchers have highlighted that physicians and nurses often hold negative attitudes towards older patients18 and are less empathic towards them compared to younger patients. There are several consequences of holding negative attitudes towards older adults that could have deleterious effects on the lives of older people. For instance, ageism among health professionals posits notable danger because it may impact the quality of care provided to older patients.19-20 In fact, healthcare professionals agree that older adults receive a lower quality of care compared to younger patient populations.21 Many physicians and nurses perceive older adults as weak, demented, ill, and intolerant,18 and they often report that they do not like working with older individuals.22-23 Consequently, efforts to reduce ageism among healthcare professionals could have a major positive impact on the quality of life of older adults, as they could contribute to giving aging individuals access to a better quality of healthcare.19-20

Research related to combatting ageist beliefs has shown that gaining education related to ageing is associated with increased empathy and enhanced attitudes towards older adults.24-26 This is the case especially among students in the helping professions,25,27 but research studies on the link between attitudes towards older adults and intervention strategies are limited.28 In particular, experiential learning (e.g., service learning, internship, and field placement) can lead to lower ageism.29 In this regard, medical students who completed a geriatric clerkship as part of their internal medicine rotation reported a more positive attitude towards older adults.30 Furthermore, in two investigations on this topic, Kumagai31,32 used a story-telling approach to improve attitudes among medical students as students paid home visits to the family of a patient affected with chronic conditions during the two years of their program. After a scheduled home visit and listening to the volunteers’ life stories, students participated in small group discussions with their instructor in the first study; in the follow-up study on the effects of “diabetes stories”, students were asked to reflect on how their understanding of chronic illness differed from knowledge gained through lectures and textbooks. Qualitative analyses of both studies showed that this intervention promoted a better understanding of chronic illness and its management.

Getting individuals to understand, connect with, and and feel empathy towards older adults could be the key to reducing ageism. Regarding having empathy towards older adults, researchers have documented that perspective-taking and empathy are mediators of the relationship between intergroup contacts (like those between young adults and older adults) and prejudice.33 Others have noted a relationship between empathy and reduced ageist beliefs.13 Furthermore, empathy has been shown to be impacted by media exposure. For example, emotional responses to film characters have been studied by Kincaid,34 who postulated that an empathic emotional response to film viewing is a motivational force that could make viewers reconceptualize the central problems depicted in a film. In line with this conceptualization, researchers35 showed video clips of encounters with medical patients (not specifically geriatric patients) to medical students assigned to the experimental condition. This viewing led to higher empathy scores compared to the scores obtained by the control participants, who watched a neutral film.

The present study was intended to shed light on the linkages between a documentary film-based intervention, empathy, and ageism towards older adults. Currently, no published study is available on the anti-bias effects of a documentarybased intervention targeting older adults’ pain, suffering, and resilience. Building on the work of Hojat et al35 and of Kumagai et al31-32, the current study was conducted with the intent to investigate whether a documentary-style, anti-ageism film that highlights the resilience of older adults and their ability to function despite untreated or under-treated physical pain could lead to experiencing a positive reconceptualization of older adults among young viewers. Utilizing a film-based intervention instead of in-person training or experiential learning has the added advantage of reaching a wider audience with minimal effort or funding compared to in-person training. Furthermore, a mediabased intervention has the potential to act as a surrogate or proxy for a one-on-one, in-person experience with older individuals, which has been shown to increase empathy and reduce ageism towards older adults.

In this pilot randomized controlled trial (RCT), in line with the previously summarized literature, the authors expected that viewing an original documentary film featuring resilient seniors living with physical pain, as well as experts explaining the difficulties associated with treating physical pain in older age, would shed light on this topic and possibly improve attitudes and empathy towards older adults. It was hypothesized that participants who viewed this film would report significantly lower ageism and higher empathy scores when compared to participants assigned to the control condition (i.e., the viewing of a neutral documentary of the same duration).

METHOD

Participants

Research participants were 77 undergraduate students aged from 18-29 years, from a diverse state university, who volunteered for the study to obtain course credit; 38 participants were assigned to the experimental group and 39 participants were assigned to the control group. The study was open to any undergraduate student enrolled in the research participant pool, with the exception of students who had been enrolled in courses related to the study of older adults (e.g., gerontology), in order to reduce the chances of recruiting students who were particularly well-disposed towards older adult populations. The sample was composed predominantly of women (nwomen=57 and nmen=20) and was ethnically diverse, reflecting the composition of the psychology department and the vast ethnic diversity of the university’s student population; aggregate and group demographic information is contained in Table 1.

Table 1: Demographic Characteristics of the Combined Sample (N=77) and of Each Group (Experimental n=38, Control n=39).
Demographic Variable Overall Experimental Control
N Statistic N Statistic N Statistic
Age (Range is 18-29years)
Mean 19.403 19.158 19.641
Standard Deviation 1.873 1.128 2.378
Gender
Women 57 74.0% 30 78.9% 27 69.2%
Men 20 26.0% 8 21.1% 12 30.8%
Classification
Freshman 39 50.6% 21 55.3% 18 46.2%
Sophomore 24 31.2% 13 34.2% 11 28.2%
Junior 9 11.7% 3 7.9% 6 15.4%
Senior 5 6.5% 1 2.6% 4 10.3%
Race/Ethnic Background
White/Caucasian 10 13.0% 5 13.2% 5 12.8%
White/Mixed 2 2.6% 2 5.3% 0 0.0%
Black/African American 3 3.9% 2 5.3% 1 2.6%
Black/African American Mixed 1 1.3% 1 2.6% 0 0.0%
Hispanic/Latino 48 62.3% 22 57.9% 26 66.7%
Asian/Pacific Islander 9 11.7% 4 10.5% 5 12.8%
Other 4 5.2% 2 5.3% 2 5.1%
Marital Status
Single 72 93.5% 36 94.7% 36 92.3%
Married 2 2.6% 0 0.0% 2 5.1%
Living with Significant Other 3 3.9% 2 5.3% 1 2.6%
Employment Status
Not Employed 47 61.0% 21 55.3% 26 66.7%
Part-time 25 32.5% 15 39.5% 10 25.6%
Full-time 5 6.5% 2 5.3% 3 7.7%
Total Income
Less than $20,000 21 27.3% 11 28.9% 10 25.6%
$20,000 – $39,000 29 37.7% 13 34.2% 16 41.0%
$40,000 – $59,000 7 9.1% 4 10.5% 3 7.7%
$60,000 – $79,000 6 7.8% 4 10.5% 2 5.1%
$80,000 – $99,000 5 6.5% 1 2.6% 4 10.3%
$100,000 and above 9 11.7% 5 13.2% 4 10.3%
Religion that influences you
Christianity 51 66.2% 27 71.1% 24 61.5%
Judaism 1 1.3% 0 0.0% 1 2.6%
Islam 3 3.9% 3 7.9% 0 0.0%
Hinduism 1 1.3% 0 0.0% 1 2.6%
Other 21 27.3% 8 21.1% 13 33.3%

Materials

Questionnaires:

The authors utilized validated scales that measure both empathy and ageism as well as a questionnaire covering demographic information about the participants. The demographics questionnaire has been utilized by the first author in several prior research projects and asks questions pertaining to age, gender, ethnicity, education, marital status, employment, and religion. Empathy was quantified using the Jefferson Scale of Empathy (JSE) Health Professions Student (HPS) version. The version of the JSE-HPS utilized in the present study is a modified 17-item version that was designed to measure empathy in university students within healthcare fields (see Williams, Brown, Boyle, & Dousek36 for details on the modification). The JSE-HPS items are measured on a Likert scale, with response choices ranging from 1 “Strongly Disagree” to 7 “Strongly Agree”. Williams and colleagues36 identified two subscales within the JSE-HPS: Compassionate Care and Perspective Taking. The Compassionate Care subscale contains 9 items, with statements like “I believe that emotion has no place in the treatment of medical illness” (reverse coded). The Perspective Taking subscale contains 8 items, with statements such as “Healthcare providers should try to stand in their patients’ shoes when providing care to them”. The JSE-HPS total showed acceptable reliability in the current sample (Cronbach’s α=0.803) as did the Perspective Taking subscale (Cronbach’s α=0.809). However, the Compassionate Care subscale had a slightly sub-optimal reliability (Cronbach’s α=0.751). Ageism was measured using North and Fiske’s Succession, Identity and Consumption (SIC) measure of ageism.37 The SIC is a 20-item scale designed to quantify intergenerational-tension; it was developed and validated on both university and non-university participants. Its items are measured on a Likert scale, and choices range from 1 “Strongly Disagree” to 6 “Strongly Agree”. This measure was designed to capture the three subscales identified in the title of the scale: Succession, Identity and Consumption. The Succession subscale contains 8 items, with statements like “Most older people don’t know when to make way for younger people”. The Identity subscale contains 5 items, with statements such as “Older people typically should not go to places where younger people hang out”. Finally, the Consumption subscale contains 7 items, with statements like “Doctors spend too much time treating sickly older people”. The SIC total and all three subscales showed acceptable levels of reliability in the current sample (SIC Total: Cronbach’s α=0.886; Succession: Cronbach’s α=0.842; Identity: Cronbach’s α=0.847; Consumption: Cronbach’s α=0.821).

Documentary films:

This study’s first and the second authors created a 90-minute documentary titled “Understanding Pain in Older Age”, which highlights the challenging nature of treating pain in older adults. The film features interviews with older individuals who share their personal stories about a) living with chronic pain due to health conditions, and in some cases b) being unable to take enough or any pain medication to successfully manage their pain, for a variety of reasons such as liver problems. Additionally, it covers interviews with health professionals, including pain experts who discuss challenges of managing physical pain in older age. This documentary has been featured at independent film festivals and has garnered recognition for its social impact efforts regarding pain and the aging adult population. The documentary film for the neutral condition was a nature documentary titled “Kiwi Country New Zealand”; it features several natural locations of New Zealand. This documentary was chosen because 1) when additional footage on New Zealand parks (available as bonus footage on the DVD) was added to it, the film had the same length as the pain documentary, 2) it won similar awards at independent film festivals, and 3) its content is thematically neutral relative to older adults and ageism.

Procedures

This investigation was approved by the Institutional Review Board governing research conducted by the first author. The study’s introduction, experiment, and debriefing were conducted in a large sized classroom that was equipped with a film projector, tables and chairs. In order to test whether watching a pain documentary could impact ageism and empathy, the authors created two experimental groups by randomizing participants (via tossing a coin) into either the experimental (i.e., pain documentary viewing) or control group (i.e., neutral documentary viewing), thus creating two randomly equivalent groups prior to treatment (see Table 1 for a list of demographic information by group). Small groups of experimental and control participants participated at separate times; the pre- and post-test data collection as well as film viewing all occurred in the same room for all participants. After a general introduction and informed consent process, students were administered a pretest consisting of the three questionnaires (i.e., demographics, empathy, and ageism) described above. Each group of students then proceeded to watch their respective 90-minute documentary film; afterwards, participants were asked to complete a post-test survey that contained the same ageism and empathy questionnaires filled out in the pre-test. Once the post-test was completed, participants were debriefed and the study was concluded. As this was a pilot study to investigate the possible use of a documentary film as an intervention method, participants were not probed for suspicion or demand characteristics following treatment/exposure to the film.

Analytic Strategy

Prior to performing any inferential analyses on the study’s variables, the analytical plan included calculating descriptive statistics (i.e., means, standard deviations, and frequencies) for both the Empathy and the Ageism scales as well as for the demographic items. Next, the authors planned to conduct Profile Analyses (Multivariate Mixed ANOVAs) to verify that an interaction effect was present, namely, whether scores for Ageism and Empathy moved significantly and in the correct direction for the experimental but not the control group. Subsequent to the multivariate analysis, the analytical strategy included conducting paired samples t-tests to further compare pre- and post-ageism and empathy scores separately in each group (control vs. treatment).

RESULTS

The collected data was analyzed using the SPSS-PC software version 22. All statistical tests employed an alpha level of 0.05. Profile analyses (i.e., multivariate mixed ANOVAs) were carried out for the total and subscale scores for both Empathy and Ageism measures with Time (Pre vs. Post) as the within-subjects factor and Group (treatment vs. control) as the between-subjects factor. Profile analyses provide tests for aggregate change over time (i.e., ignoring group membership), group differences(footnote1) (i.e., collapsing the pre- and post-test scores), and a test for the interaction between Time and Group. Prior to conducting the analyses, no univariate or multivariate outliers were identified in the dataset (α=0.001).38 Additionally, data screening revealed that the assumptions of normality, homogeneity of variancecovariance matrices, linearity, and multicollinearity were met, thus the dataset was suitable for the planned analyses.

Empathy

A profile analysis was performed on the basis of the total JSEHPS scores; the findings indicated that the univariate effect for the Group was not significant (F(1,75)=3.161, p=0.079, partial η2 =0.040). Results for the multivariate tests indicated a significant difference for the main effect of Time (Wilk’s λ=0.877, F(1,75)=10.557, p=0.002, partial η2 =0.123). The 2-way interaction between Time and Group was also significant (Wilk’s λ=0.933, F(1,75)=5.389, p=0.023, partial η2 =0.067; see Figure 1). Analyses of each of the subscales indicated that, for Compassionate Care, the effect for Time was significant (Wilk’s λ=0.924, F(1,75)=6.186, p=0.015, partial η2 =0.076), but the effects of both Group (F(1,75)=2.266, p=0.136, partial η2 =0.029) and the interaction (Wilk’s λ=0.981, F(1,75)=1.474, p=0.228, partial η2 =0.019) were not significant. Regarding the Perspective Taking subscale, the interaction was significant (Wilk’s λ=0.950, F(1,75)=3.964, p=0.050, partial η2 =0.050). However, the effects of Group (F(1,75)=2.142, p=0.147, η2 =0.028) and Time (Wilk’s λ=0.953, F(1,75)=3.704, p=0.058, partial η2 =0.047) were not significant, as illustrated in Figure 1.

Figure 1: Average Pre- and Post-Test Total and Subscale Scores for the Empathy and Ageism Measures for the Control and Experimental Groups.

PCSOJ-3-127Fig1

Ageism

A profile analysis was performed on the basis of the total SIC scores; the results indicated that the univariate effect for Group was not significant (F(1,75)=0.128, p=0.721, η2 =0.002). Results for the multivariate tests indicated no significant difference for the main effect of Time (Wilk’s λ=0.968, F(1,75)=2.472, p=0.120, partial η2 =0.032). The 2-way interaction between Time and Group was not significant (Wilk’s λ=0.994, F(1,75)=0.482, p=0.490, partial η2 =0.006; see Figure 1). Analyses of each of the subscales resulted in the following findings: 1) Succession: the Time effect was significant (Wilk’s λ=0.950, F(1,75)=3.966, p=.050, partial η2 =0.050); however, the Group (F(1,75)=0.007, p=0.782, η2 =0.001) and interaction (Wilk’s λ=0.990, F(1,75)=0.728, p=0.396, partial η2 =0.010) effects were not significant; 2) Identity: Group (F(1,75)=0.022, p=.883, partial η2 =0.0002), Time (Wilk’s λ=1.000, F(1,75)=0.003, p=0.955, partial η2 =0.00004), and interaction (Wilk’s λ=1.000, F(1,75)=0.032, p=0.859, partial η2 =0.0004) effects were not significant; and 3) Consumption: Group (F(1,75)=0.188, p=0.666, partial η2 =0.002), Time (Wilk’s λ=0.994, F(1,75)=.425, p=0.517, partial η2 =0.042), and interaction (Wilk’s λ=0.958, F(1,75)=3.253, p=0.075, partial η2 =0.042) effects were not significant (see Figure 1).


Footnote

1. This is a univariate test because each participant’s score used in this test is an average across the within-subjects factors.


Paired Sample t-test Results

Table 2 illustrates the descriptive statistics for the total and subscale scores for Empathy and Ageism, as well as the correlational values and the results of the paired samples t-tests (i.e., post-test minus pre-test score). Results of the paired sample t-tests on the total scores indicated that there was a significant mean difference in Empathy scores for the experimental group (t(37)=2.999, p=0.005), but not for the control group (t(38)=1.175, p=0.247). However, there were no significant mean differences for Ageism scores for either the experimental (t(37)=-1.417, p=0.165) or control (t(38)=-0.725, p=0.473) group. The significant difference between the pre- and post-test scores on Empathy for the experimental group was primarily driven by a significant change in the post-test scores on the Perspective Taking subscale (t(37)=2.191, p=0.035). The change on the Compassionate Care subscale for the experimental group was right on the cusp of significance as well (t(37)=2.011, p=0.052). Table 2 does indicate an effect for the Succession subscale of the Ageism measure in that, although the overall scale did not show a significant change for either group, there was a significant decrease for the control group on this one subscale (t(38)=-2.299, p=0.027). Additionally, Table 2 shows that the correlations between pre- and post-test scores on both the Empathy and Ageism scales were very high (indicating that participants responded consistently across time) for the control group participants, while for the experimental group the correlations were often considerably lower compared to the control group. For example, for the overall measure of Empathy, in the experimental group the correlation was relatively low (r=0.468) when compared to the correlation on the same measure in the control group (r=0.914).

Table 2: Descriptive Statistics and t-Test Results for the Total and Subscales Scores of the Empathy and Ageism Scales.
Pre-test Post-test Pearson Post-, Pre- Mean Difference
Scale Group M SD M SD n r 95% CI t-value df sig.

Empathy

(JSE-HSP)

Total Control 87.436 12.219 88.462 13.432 39 0.914* -0.741 2.793 1.175 38 0.247
Experimental 89.500 13.838 95.658 9.721 38 0.468* 1.998 10.318 2.999 37 0.005*
Compassionate Control 44.513 7.196 45.590 8.840 39 0.872* -0.337 2.491 1.542 38 0.131
Care Experimental 45.921 9.652 49.053 5.789 38 0.309 -0.024 6.287 2.011 37 0.052
Perspective Control 42.923 6.823 42.872 7.230 39 0.797* -1.507 1.404 -0.071 38 0.944
Taking Experimental 43.579 8.617 46.605 6.804 38 0.410* 0.227 5.825 2.191 37 0.035*
Total Control 49.487 14.133 48.692 16.579 39 0.913* -3.015 1.425 -0.725 38 0.473
Experimental 48.895 16.523 46.842 14.487 38 0.842* -4.989 0.883 -1.417 37 0.165
Ageism

(SIC)

Succession Control 25.590 8.861 24.077 10.017 39 0.912* -2.845 -0.181 -2.299 38 0.027*
Experimental 24.605 7.557 24.000 8.217 38 0.788* -2.306 1.096 -0.721 37 0.475
Identity Control 9.974 4.960 10.026 5.096 39 0.955* -0.442 0.544 0.211 38 0.834
Experimental 9.842 5.320 9.816 5.281 38 0.910* -0.765 0.713 -0.072 37 0.943
Consumption Control 13.923 4.636 14.590 6.021 39 0.812* -0.474 1.808 1.183 38 0.244
Experimental 14.447 7.199 13.026 5.206 38 0.525* -3.488 0.646 -1.393 37 0.172
CI=Confidence interval, *p<0.05.
DISCUSSION

In the present experiment, the authors examined whether viewing a pain-focused documentary could affect the perception of older adults and their many medical struggles among college students, with the intent of improving empathy and attitude towards them. Similar to previous research on the impact of exposure on empathy,13,24-27,30-32,35 the significant 2-way interaction between Group and Time found in the Profile Analysis results indicated that viewing the pain documentary did in fact impact experimental participants’ empathy scores positively and significantly. Follow-up t-test analyses were performed in order to delineate this effect; it was discovered that the impact of viewing the pain documentary was reflected in the significant changes in participants’ empathy scores, and this effect was only found in the experimental group. Further investigation into the subscales of the empathy measure revealed that what was driving the interaction for the overall empathy score was a significant change in the perspective taking subscale for the experimental group. No significant improvements in total ageism scores were recorded in either group; however, there was a significant, albeit somewhat unexplainable, improvement on the Succession subscale of the SIC for the control group. A potential explanation is that, perhaps, given that some portions of the control film were narrated by an older adult, this could have led to the result in question. In view of the effect size values for many of the nonsignificant effects, it is speculated that conducting this study on a larger sample, a process already underway, could lead to achieving additional significant improvements on the Compassionate Care subscale of the JSE-HPS as well as on the Consumption subscale of the SIC.

This study has some valuable implications for designing interventions to improve empathy and attitude towards older adults. Successful interventions may help reduce intergenerational tension, improve quality of life of older adults, and eventually provide them with better access to healthcare, work resources, and possibly even political representation. These interventions could also help decrease a growing problem of recruiting and retaining healthcare providers to geriatric professions.39 Furthermore, conducting successful interventions in this area may help our society become better prepared to meet the demands of a multigenerational society characterized by an increasingly growing geriatric population.

Limitations

This study had its own limitations. First of all, the sample size was modest (n=77), especially in view of the relatively low correlation for pre- and post-empathy scores in the experimental group, which indicated that the effect that the documentary had might have depended on additional participant information not assessed in the study. This relatively low correlation suggests that, while experimental participants increased their empathy on average after viewing the pain-focused film, their scores did not remain as consistent as they did regarding the Empathy measure in the control group (or even for the ageism measure in the experimental group). In other words, some participants’ empathy did increase relative to their starting position but, for others, empathy either did not change or changed in the opposite direction, which is why the correlation was somewhat low. To investigate the above-mentioned issue further, a larger sample is needed in order to include additional variables in the analyses such as age (even within a university student population, as the current study had a relatively wide age range of 18-29 years), gender, as well as regular interaction with older adults as potential moderators of the effect of the intervention on the outcome variables.

Another limitation, common to many studies of this kind, is the fact that the post-test occurred immediately after film viewing. First, having achieved a significant change in empathy scores in a short time is a remarkable accomplishment of the intervention, although it does not allow to ascertain whether these changes will remain after the participants leave the study, nor does it allow for the investigation of whether the empathy changes found immediately in this study will lead to changes in ageism later. Secondly, utilizing a pre-test and post-test in a rather quick succession makes it difficult to confirm whether the changes were in fact real. For instance, using a pre-test and post-test of the same measures in a short amount of time may pose a risk of participants remembering their answers to the pretest (e.g., urging them not to change their answers) or guessing what answers researchers may expect from participants – this is a shortcoming of randomized controlled research of this kind in general. In a similar way, there could be an issue of demand characteristics, as participants who are given pre- and post-tests that relate to the intervention may alter their response behavior to affirm what the researchers are investigating. However, the fact that this study’s participants changed on empathy and not on ageism after viewing a documentary featuring older adults makes this concern less critical.

Moreover, considering that two of this article’s authors are the creators of the experimental film, this could create inherent bias. This bias could stem from the fact that some participants might have recognized the film as being made by the team of a CSUN professor and could manifest, for instance, in the authors’ interpretation of outcomes and in how their research team interacted with the participants. Additionally, the study is limited in its generalizability because the demographic makeup of the current sample is not necessarily comparable to other samples of university students. For instance, women and Hispanic-Latino/a students were naturally oversampled in the current study, as this reflects the gender and ethnic composition of the undergraduate student population utilized in the study. Furthermore, the present findings cannot be easily generalized to students from other dissimilar educational institutions nor to non-students of the same age group in general.

Future Directions

As a pilot study with promising results, the current investigation is the first step of many towards truly understanding the impact that a documentary film can have on young individuals’ attitudes towards older adults. To begin with, future research in this area should ideally be conducted on a larger scale in order to include and parse out additional variables that could be impacting the effect of the intervention on potential outcomes. Interested researchers should not only investigate the potential moderating effects of demographic group membership (e.g., age and gender) to further elucidate the cause of the inconsistent responding from pre- to post-test on some of the measures for the experimental group, but also multiple factors that are potentially related to the outcome variables. As an example of additional factors, some evidence suggests that death anxiety and ageism are highly correlated.40,41,42 Death anxiety can make young people distance themselves from older adults as a coping mechanism through which to avoid confronting their own future selves and the inevitability of their own death.16 Relating this information to the current study, it is possible that, for respondents in the treatment group who had a high death anxiety level, viewing the pain film might have triggered this anxiety and led to a decrease in empathy towards older adults. Another example of a group of variables that could be included in future studies using larger sample sizes is the impact of mass media (to be measured covering several factors) on the two outcome variables in question. For instance, as reported earlier, research indicates that young adults are bombarded with mass media messages that lead them to internalize 1) youthful ideals, with beauty and youth being idolized and becoming older being related to physical and cognitive impairments as well as dependence,7-8 and 2) ageist messages, as young people are told that older adults are economic burdens to society.12 Future studies could cover the extent of research participants’ internalization of these types of messages in order to explore their mediating or moderating impact on the effects of interventions like the one tested herein.

Moreover, although a randomized controlled trial design was used in the current study, researchers could consider implementing a variety of methodological improvements (e.g., manipulation checks, probes for demand characteristics during debriefing, varying times between pre- and post-tests, implementing statistical controls, utilizing more sensitive outcomes measures) in order to ensure significant findings that are meaningful. For instance, interested scholars could actively attempt to reduce participant-related artifacts that could impact the study (e.g., demand characteristics) and include questions that would 1) verify whether the intervention was fully recognized by the participant (manipulation check), 2) investigate whether the participant was suspicious of minor deceptions used to mask the point of the study, and/or 3) assess participant’s demand characteristics (e.g., “good-participant” effect) in order to fully address them during the analysis phase. In view of the limitations of the pre-test and post-tests timing mentioned, researchers could purposely vary the length of time between the pre- and post-tests in order to increase the generalizability of the effect and limit the impact of phenomena like carry-over effects. Furthermore, in the current study, it was not possible to parcel out the effects of viewing the painful film on specific aspects of empathy related to the intervention (e.g., empathy towards pain vs. towards older individuals vs. towards older adults in pain), another common shortcoming of studies of this kind. Future research could also include the utilization of outcome measures that are sensitive enough or broad enough in scope for a more nuanced investigation into how participants’ empathy and ageism were impacted by the intervention.

Conclusive Comments

This preliminary study, which could be viewed as an anti-bias educational exercise, is a promising first step towards testing whether film-based interventions can increase empathy and decrease age-related bias against older adults.

ACKNOWLEDGMENT

This research was supported by a grant from the National Institute of General Medical Sciences, award number 5SC3GM094075, Luciana Laganá, Principal Investigator. The content of this article does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institute of General Medical Sciences and is solely the responsibility of the authors.

CONFLICTS OF INTEREST

The authors declared that they have no conflicts of interest.

1. North MS, Fiske ST. Modern attitudes toward older adults in the aging world: A cross-cultural meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. 2015; 141(5): 993-1021. doi: 10.1037/a0039469

2. Cuddy AJ, Norton MI, Fiske ST. This old stereotype: The pervasiveness and persistence of the elderly stereotype. J Soc Issues. 2005; 61(2): 267-285. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.2005.00405.x

3. Butler RN. Why survive? Being Old in America. New York, USA: Harper & Row; 1975: 12.

4. Nelson TD. The age of ageism. J Soc Issues. 2016; 72(1): 191- 198. doi: 10.1111/josi.12162

5. Nelson TD. Ageism: Prejudice against our feared future self. J Soc Issues. 2005; 61(2): 207-221. doi: 10.1111/j.1540- 4560.2005.00402.x

6. Ray S, Sharp E, Abrams D. Ageism: A Benchmark of Public Attitudes in Britain. London, England: Age Concern: 2006: 74.

7. Blazer DG. Guest Editorial The prevalence of depressive symptoms. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2002; 57(3): M150-M151. doi: 10.1093/gerona/57.3.M150

8. Abeles N, Cooley S, Deitch IM, et al. What practitioners should know about working with older adults. Prof Psychol Res Pr. 1998; 29(5): 413-427.

9. Haboush A, Warren CS, Benuto L. Beauty, ethnicity, and age: Does internalization of mainstream media ideals influence attitudes towards older adults? Sex roles. 2012; 66(9-10): 668- 676. doi: 10.1007/s11199-011-0102-6

10. Stone SD. Disability, dependence, and old age: Problematic constructions. Can J Aging. 2003; 22(1): 59-67. doi: 10.1017/s0714980800003731

11. Palmore E. Ageism: Negative and Positive. New York, USA: Springer; 1990.

12. Lovell M. Caring for the elderly: Changing perceptions and attitudes. J Vasc Nurs. 2006; 24(1), 22-26. doi: 10.1016/j. jvn.2005.11.001

13. Allan LJ, Johnson JA, Emerson SD. The role of individual difference variables in ageism. Pers Individ Dif. 2014; 59: 32- 37. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2013.10.027

14. Eisenberg N, Miller PA. Empathy, sympathy, and altruism: Empirical and conceptual links. In: Eisenberg N, Strayer J, eds. Empathy and Its Development. New York, USA: Cambridge University Press; 1987: 292-316.

15. Escalas JE, Stern BB. Sympathy and empathy: Emotional responses to advertising dramas. J Consum Res. 2003; 29(4): 566-578. doi: 10.1086/346251

16. Packer DJ, Chasteen AL. Looking to the future: How possible aged selves influence prejudice toward older adults. Soc Cognition. 2006; 24(3): 218-247. doi: 10.1521/soco.2006.24.3.218

17. Jönson H. We will be different! Ageism and the temporal construction of old age. Gerontologist. 2013; 53(2): 198-204. doi: 10.1093/geront/gns066

18. Polat Ü, Karadağ A, Ülger Z, Demir N. Nurses’ and physicians’ perceptions of older people and attitudes towards older people: Ageism in a hospital in Turkey. Contemp Nurse. 2014; 48(1): 88-97. doi: 10.1080/10376178.2014.11081930

19. Abdou CM, Fingerhut AW, Jackson JS, Wheaton F. Healthcare stereotype threat in older adults in the Health and Retirement Study. Am J Prev Med. 2016; 50(2): 191-198. doi: 10.1016/j. amepre.2015.07.034

20. Courtney M, Tong S, Walsh A. Acute-care nurses’ attitudes towards older patients: A literature review. Int J Nurs Pract. 2000; 6(2): 62-69. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-172x.2000.00192.x

21. Pedersen R, Nortvedt P, Nordhaug M, et al. In quest of justice? Clinical prioritisation in healthcare for the aged. J Med Ethics. 2008; 34(4): 230-235. doi: 10.1136/jme.2006.018531

22. Berenbaum R. Motivating students in the helping professions to work with the aged. Educ Gerontol. 2000; 26(1): 83-95. doi: 10.1080/036012700267411

23. Litwin H. The professional standing of work with elderly persons among social work trainees. Br J Soc Work. 1994; 24(1): 53-69. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.bjsw.a056040

24. Levy SR. Toward reducing ageism: PEACE (Positive Education about Aging and Contact Experiences) model. Gerontologist. 2016; pii: gnw116. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnw116

25. Wu LL. A cross-sectional survey of student nurses’ attitudes and knowledge about older people. Chinese J Gerontol. 2011; 31(5): 848-850.

26. Wells Y, Foreman P, Gething L, Petralia W. Nurses’ attitudes toward aging and older adults. J Gerontol Nurs. 2004; 30(9): 5-9. doi: 10.3928/0098-9134-20040901-04

27. Lambrinou E, Sourtzi P, Kalokerinou A, Lemonidou C. Attitudes and knowledge of the Greek nursing students towards older people. Nurse Educ Today. 2009; 29(6): 617-622. doi: 10.1016/j.nedt.2009.01.011

28. Liu YE, Norman IJ, While AE. Nurses’ attitudes towards older people: A systematic review. Int J Nurs Stud. 2013; 50(9): 1271-1282. doi: 10.1016/j.ijnurstu.2012.11.021

29. Bergman EJ, Erickson MA, Simons JN. Attracting and training tomorrow’s gerontologists: what drives student interest in aging? Educ Gerontol. 2014; 40(3): 172-185. doi: 10.1080/03601277.2013.802184

30. Nguyen AL, Duthie EA, Denson KM, Franco J, Duthie EH. Positioning medical students for the geriatric imperative: Using geriatrics to effectively teach medicine. Gerontol Geriatr Educ. 2013; 34(4): 342-353. doi: 10.1080/02701960.2013.809714

31. Kumagai AK. A conceptual framework for the use of illness narratives in medical education. Acad Med. 2008; 83(7): 653- 658. doi: 10.1097/ACM.0b013e3181782e17

32. Kumagai AK, Murphy EA, Ross PT. Diabetes stories: Use of patient narratives of diabetes to teach patient-centered care. Adv Health Sci Educ Theory Pract. 2009; 14(3): 315-326. doi: 10.1007/s10459-008-9123-5

33. Tam T, Hewstone M, Harwood J, Voci A, Kenworthy J. Intergroup contact and grandparent–grandchild communication: The effects of self-disclosure on implicit and explicit biases against older people. Group Process Intergroup Relat. 2006; 9(3): 413- 429. doi: 10.1177/1368430206064642

34. Kincaid DL. Drama, emotion, and cultural convergence. Commun Theory. 2002; 12(2): 136-152. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-28 85.2002.tb00263.x

35. Hojat M, Axelrod D, Spandorfer J, Mangione S. Enhancing and sustaining empathy in medical students. Med Teach. 2013; 35(12): 996-1001. doi: 10.3109/0142159X.2013.802300

36. Williams B, Brown T, Boyle M, Dousek S. Psychometric testing of the jefferson scale of empathy health profession students’ version with Australian paramedic students. Nurs Health Sci. 2013; 15(1): 45-50. doi: 10.1111/j.1442-2018.2012.00719.x

37. North MS, Fiske ST. A prescriptive intergenerational-tension ageism scale: Succession, identity, and consumption (SIC). Psychol Assess. 2013; 25(3): 706-713. doi: 10.1037/a0032367

38. Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS. Using Multivariate Statistics. 5th ed. Boston, MA, USA: Pearson/Allyn & Bacon; 2007.

39. Brown J, Nolan M, Davies S, Nolan J, Keady J. Transforming students’ views of gerontological nursing: Realising the potential of ‘enriched’ environments of learning and care: A multi-method longitudinal study. Int J Nurs Stud. 2008; 45(8): 1214-1232. doi: 10.1016/j.ijnurstu.2007.07.002

40. Martens A, Goldenberg JL, Greenberg J. A terror management perspective on ageism. J Soc Issues. 2005; 61(2): 223-239. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.2005.00403.x

41. Martens A, Greenberg J, Schimel J, Landau MJ. Ageism and death: effects of mortality salience and perceived similarity to elders on reactions to elderly people. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2004; 30(12): 1524-1536. doi: 10.1177/0146167204271185

42. Greenberg J, Schimel J, Martens A. Ageism: Denying the face of the future. In: TD Nelson, ed. Ageism: Stereotyping and Prejudice against Older Persons. Cambridge, MA, USA: MIT Press; 2002: 27-48.doi:10.7551/mitpress/1157.003.0005

LATEST ARTICLES

Practical Pointers for Drug Development and Medical Affairs

Gerald L. Klein*, Roger E. Morgan, Shabnam Vaezzadeh, Burak Pakkal and Pavle Vukojevic

doi.

10.17140/CTPOJ-7-125

Prevalence and Risk Factors of Subclinical Mastitis of Goats in Banadir Region, Somalia

Omar M. Salah*, Yasin H. Sh-Hassan, Moktar O. S. Mohamed, Mohamed A. Yusuf and Abas S. A. Jimale

doi.10.17140/VMOJ-9-184

Use of Black Soldier Fly (Hermetia illucens) Prepupae Reared on Organic Waste

Maggot Debridement Therapy: A Natural Solution for Wound Healing

Isayas A. Kebede*, Haben F. Gebremeskel and Gelan D. Dahesa,

doi.10.17140/VMOJ-9-183

Figure 11. Risk Map for the Introduction of Ruminant Diseases at Borders

Ovine Network in Morocco: Epizootics Spread Prevention and Identification of the At-Risk Areas for “Peste des Petits Ruminants” and “Foot and Mouth Disease”

Yassir Lezaar*, Mehdi Boumalik, Youssef Lhor, Moha El-Ayachi, Abelilah Araba and Mohammed Bouslikhane

doi.

10.17140/EPOJ-8-131

The Impact of Family Dynamics on Palliative Care at the End-of-Life

Neil A. Nijhawan*, Rasha Mustafa and Aqeela Sheikh

doi.10.17140/PMHCOJ-10-154

Long-Term Follow-Up After Laparoscopic Radical Prostatectomy for Localized and Locally Advanced Prostate Cancer

Shrenik J. Shah*, Abhishek Jha, Chirag Davara, Rushi Mistry and Kapil Kachhadiya

doi.

10.17140/UAOJ-7-147

LATEST ARTICLES

Pie Chart Showing Overall Proportions of Diagnostic Category of FNAC, JUMC

Retrospective Study

2024 Apr

Abel Tefera*, Lemlem Terefe and Kitesa Biresa
Prevalence (%) of Types of Anthropometric Failure among Previous and Present Studied Tribal Children

Original Research, peer reviewed

2024 Apr

Biswajit Mahapatra and Kaushik Bose*

Opinion

2024 Apr

Gerald L. Klein*, Roger E. Morgan, Shabnam Vaezzadeh, Burak Pakkal and Pavle Vukojevic